Let Y=f(X), where X is any particular articulatory parameter (tongue tip position, for example), and Y is any particular perceptual parameter (perceived frequency of the peak in the acoustic spectrum, for example). Like any nonlinear relation, f(X) has regions of low slope (|df/dX| small) and regions of high slope (|df/dX| large). Values of Y drawn from a high-slope region are unstable, in the sense that a small change in X causes a large change in Y; values of Y drawn from a low-slope region are conversely stable, in that they are little perturbed by large changes in X. Stevens proposed in 19681 that the stability of low-slope regions makes them more likely to be chosen as discrete linguistic units (phonemes) by the languages of the world, and that the distinction between any pair of phonemes tends similarly to occur across an unstable high-slope boundary region. Examples include
Quantal theory is supported by a theory of language change, developed in collaboration with Jay Keyser, which postulates the existence of redundant or enhancement features.7
It is quite common, in language, to find a pair of phonemes that differ in two features simultaneously. In English, for example, "thin" and "sin" differ in both the place of articulation of the fricative (teeth versus alveolar ridge), and in its loudness (nonstrident versus strident). Similarly, "tell" and "dell" differ in both the voicing of the initial consonant, and in its aspiration (the /t/ in "tell" is immediately followed by a puff of air, like a short /h/ between the plosive and the vowel). In many cases, native speakers have strong and mistaken intuition about the relative importance of the two distinctions, e.g., speakers of English believe that "thin" versus "sin" is a place of articulation difference, even though the loudness difference is more perceptible. Stevens, Keyser and Kawasaki8 proposed that such redundant features evolve as an enhancement9 of an otherwise weak acoustic distinction, in order to improve the robustness of the language's phonological system.
K.N. Stevens (1968). The quantal nature of speech: evidence from articulatory-acoustic data. https://books.google.com/books?id=6KYeAQAAMAAJ ↩
"Acoustical Society of America Gold Medal Award, 1995: Kenneth N. Stevens". Archived from the original on 2007-06-27. Retrieved 2013-07-04. https://web.archive.org/web/20070627024218/http://asa.aip.org/encomia/gold/stevens.html ↩
K.N. Stevens (1971). ""Airflow and Turbulence Noise for Fricative and Stop Consonants: Static Considerations," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 50(4):1180-1192". http://asadl.org/jasa/resource/1/jasman/v50/i4B/p1180_s1?isAuthorized=no ↩
K.N. Stevens (1987). "Relational Properties as Perceptual Correlates of Phonetic Features," Proc. Eleventh Int. Conf. Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS) 4:352-356. https://books.google.com/books?id=3zDVAAAAMAAJ ↩
I. Lehiste and G.E. Peterson (1961). ""Transitions, Glides, and Diphthongs," J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 33(3):268-277". http://asadl.org/jasa/resource/1/jasman/v33/i3/p268_s1?isAuthorized=no ↩
K.N. Stevens and S.J. Keyser (1989). ""Primary Features and their Enhancement in Consonants," Language 65(1):81-106". Language. 65 (1): 81–106. doi:10.2307/414843. JSTOR 414843. /wiki/Doi_(identifier) ↩
K.N. Stevens and S.J. Keyser and H. Kawasaki (1986). "Toward a Phonetic and Phonological Theory of Redundant Features," in "Invariance and Variability in Speech Processes," Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 426-463. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 9780898595451. 9780898595451 ↩