Although the term "retrieval-induced forgetting" was first used in 1994, it was described in an earlier review by Robert A. Bjork in terms of suppressing memories that become active but are not relevant for a given situation. Bjork described a study by Neely and Durgunoğlu who found that participants were slower at recognizing words when they were shown a related word immediately beforehand. The researchers hypothesized that participants were actually trying to suppress the related words during recognition, as it was irrelevant and unhelpful information in completing the recognition task. Since the time of Bjork's original description of this phenomenon, an important portion of his cognitive psychology research performed with members of the Bjork Learning and Forgetting Lab and the Cogfog group at UCLA has explored various aspects of retrieval-induced forgetting.
In the study phase, participants are asked to study word pairs that consist of a category name and a word that belongs to that category, an example being FRUIT–orange. Typically multiple word pairs across a number of categories, say 48 items divided equally into 8 categories, are used. Among several others, participants might study word pairs such as the following:
A subset of the items are tested using a fill-in-the-blank test. For a given item, the participant is shown the category name and the first two letters of a studied word from that category (e.g. METAL–ir______). Participants are instructed to fill in the blank with an appropriate studied word from that category. Participants will typically practice remembering the items multiple times. The retrieval-practice phase splits items into three different types that are of interest during the final test, and are often denoted using the following notation:
After retrieval practice, participants are given a final test, where they are asked to remember all studied items. The goal of the test phase is to assess whether prior retrieval practice affects how well the unpracticed–related words are remembered compared to the unpracticed–unrelated words. RIF is ultimately measured as the difference between the proportion of items recalled between these two types.
RIF studies have generally yielded results where, on average, unpracticed–related words are remembered less well than the baseline of unpracticed–unrelated words.
RIF studies have generally used words from basic and easily recognized categories, but the effects have been shown with a wide variety of stimuli:
Although the typical paradigm includes retrieval practice of previously studied words, some studies have shown RIF even when participants were asked to retrieve something else. For instance, RIF has occurred even when participants generated new, unstudied items from previously studied categories in what is called "extra-list retrieval practice" or "semantic generation." In a method called "impossible retrieval practice," RIF has also been observed when participants were asked to generate a word for a category, even though one did not actually exist. RIF is still observed at final test in cases where successful retrieval is not possible, such as one where having studied a number of fruits a participant is asked to generate a word given a cue resembling FRUIT–wu. Some studies have also examined the effect on RIF when participants, instead of being asked to perform retrieval practice, are given additional study trials making them restudy the material instead of remembering prior information. In these cases, participants have failed to show any RIF effects.
Because RIF is an effect related to the accessibility of information, researchers have studied whether it persists in populations that have certain disorders related to memory. In one study of students diagnosed with ADHD, the degree of RIF observed compared to a control group depended on the kind of final test used. When using a category-cued test, there were no differences in RIF compared to a control group. However, when a category-plus-stem-cued test was used, participants with ADHD, on average, showed less RIF than controls. Patients with depression show no RIF compared to controls when using a category-plus-stem-cued test. Patients with schizophrenia show comparable RIF effects to control groups under a category-cued test, but reduced RIF using a recognition test.
The effects of studying or remembering information in groups has also been examined. When groups of individuals experience an event together, and then jointly remember the event, conditions are created for retrieval induced forgetting to occur. Selective remembering in the conversation induces both Speakers and Listeners in the conversation to forget unmentioned, but related to the mentioned, memories (Rp-) to a larger degree than unmentioned, but unrelated to the mentioned memories (NRp). This effect is called socially-shared RIF, and can even occur with flashbulb memories, which are memories of circumstances in which one learned about consequential events. One example of this phenomenon is when Americans influence each other's memories of the September 11th attacks by communicating with one another about these memories. More specifically, remembering a subset of details about the event causes RIF for other related, critical information about the event. Importantly, simply listening to somebody remembering her September 11 memories results in induced forgetting in one's own memories.
Of note, this particular phenomenon of SSRIF has been linked to how communities of individuals form collective memories of the past by selectively retrieving information in conversational interactions.
Many instances of forgetting are often accounted to the interference from heightened accessibility of other, associated information in memory. Researchers have described how interference may explain RIF in a number of ways. For instance the theory of blocking suggests that because practiced words are more easily remembered at test, they may occupy a "response channel" in memory and effectively prevent unpracticed–related words in memory from being remembered. Unpracticed–unrelated words would be less affected by this blocking effect because retrieval of practiced words would not block items from different categories. Other theories such as resource diffusion and response decrement are similar to blocking. These theories describe retrieval processes as a finite set of resources that cannot be distributed adequately enough to unpracticed–related items at test.
Other models of interference have been proposed that more precisely define the idea of an item's strength in memory through separating the strength of item itself in memory and the contextual information it is linked to. Such models may explain why certain strengthening methods predict RIF and others do not.
Generally speaking, inhibition theory assumes the existence of a set of processes that allows the suppression of memories. Central to the inhibition account of RIF is that access to unpracticed–related items is actively suppressed by this inhibitory process during retrieval-practice. For instance, when participants perform retrieval practice, the category cue may activate many associated items. The degree to which related, but inappropriate associates, that is unpracticed–related words, become accessible serves as a source of competition that disrupts retrieval of an appropriate response. To resolve this competition, an inhibitory process intervenes to suppress accessibility to such items. Subsequently, this suppression facilitates retrieval of an appropriate item and prevents retrieval of contextually inappropriate items. Items from unrelated categories, that is unpracticed–unrelated items, are less competitive during retrieval practice and thus, require less inhibition. At final test, the consequences of the suppression persist, and previously competitive items that were inhibited become more difficult to remember.
This reduction in accessibility is consistent with the definition of inhibition proposed by Robert A. Bjork: that inhibition is an active, direct form of suppression that serves to reduce access to one or several responses for some adaptive purpose.
Memory inhibition in reference to RIF has sometimes been likened to processes of inhibition associated with motor control, such as those responsible for baseball players stopping their swing when they anticipate a ball. Similarly, when a dominant response in memory is inappropriate, inhibitory processes must be recruited to temporarily suppress that response so that a more appropriate one can be retrieved. In addition, recent studies indicate an increase in the activity of the frontal cortex during the manifestation of the RIF phenomenon, which speaks in favor of increased cognitive control inhibiting the retrieval of related non-target information.
Individual strategies in retrieval have been considered as one way RIF might occur, in that retrieval practice may be inconsistent with the way participants remember items studied from those categories. If retrieval practice disrupts participants' memory strategy, it may affect their ability to remember particular items at final test. If participants are preparing to remember items during retrieval-practice based on some strategy, certain presentation orders may disrupt that strategy whereas others may not. For instance, when participants practice items in the same order as presented during study, RIF is lower compared to when presentation is random during retrieval practice. These results are the same even when participants are explicitly instructed to remember the order in which items are presented during study.
Anderson, Michael C. (2005). "The role of inhibitory control in forgetting unwanted memories: A consideration of three methods.". In Nobuo Ohta; Colin M. MacLeod; Bob Uttl (eds.). Dynamic Cognitive Processes. Tokyo: Springer. pp. 159–189. ISBN 978-4431239994. 978-4431239994
Sahakyan, L.; Kelley, C.M. (2002). "Contextual change account of the directed forgetting effect". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 28 (6): 1064–1072. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.28.6.1064. PMID 12450332. S2CID 16995742. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Perfect, Timothy J.; Stark, Louisa-Jayne; Tree, Jeremy J.; Moulin, Christopher J.A.; Ahmed, Lubna; Hutter, Russell (1 October 2004). "Transfer appropriate forgetting: The cue-dependent nature of retrieval-induced forgetting". Journal of Memory and Language. 51 (3): 399–417. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.06.003. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Anderson, Michael C.; Bjork, Robert A.; Bjork, Elizabeth L. (1994). "Remembering Can Cause Forgetting: Retrieval Dynamics in Long-Term Memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 20 (5): 1063–1087. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.119.3933. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.5.1063. PMID 7931095. /wiki/CiteSeerX_(identifier)
Robert A. Bjork (1989). Henry L. Roediger; Fergus I. M. Craik (eds.). Varieties of Memory and Consciousness: Essays in Honour of Endel Tulving. Psychology Press. p. 337. ISBN 978-0898599350. 978-0898599350
Neely, James H; Durgunoǧlu, Aydin Y (1985). "Dissociative episodic and semantic priming effects in episodic recognition and lexical decision tasks". Journal of Memory and Language. 24 (4): 466–489. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(85)90040-3. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
"Research | Bjork Learning and Forgetting Lab". bjorklab.psych.ucla.edu. Retrieved 2024-02-25. https://bjorklab.psych.ucla.edu/research/
Nickerson, R.S. (1984). "Retrieval inhibition from part-set cuing: A persisting enigma in memory research". Memory & Cognition. 12 (6): 531–552. doi:10.3758/BF03213342. PMID 6398394. https://doi.org/10.3758%2FBF03213342
Anderson, Michael C. (2005). "The role of inhibitory control in forgetting unwanted memories: A consideration of three methods.". In Nobuo Ohta; Colin M. MacLeod; Bob Uttl (eds.). Dynamic Cognitive Processes. Tokyo: Springer. pp. 159–189. ISBN 978-4431239994. 978-4431239994
Sahakyan, L.; Kelley, C.M. (2002). "Contextual change account of the directed forgetting effect". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 28 (6): 1064–1072. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.28.6.1064. PMID 12450332. S2CID 16995742. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Anderson, Michael C.; Spellman, Barbara A. (1 January 1995). "On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: Memory retrieval as a model case". Psychological Review. 102 (1): 68–100. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.68. PMID 7878163. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Anderson, Michael C.; Bjork, Robert A.; Bjork, Elizabeth L. (1994). "Remembering Can Cause Forgetting: Retrieval Dynamics in Long-Term Memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 20 (5): 1063–1087. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.119.3933. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.5.1063. PMID 7931095. /wiki/CiteSeerX_(identifier)
Bajo, M. Teresa; Gómez-Ariza, C.J.; Fernandez, C.; Marful, A. (September 2006). "Retrieval-induced forgetting in perceptually driven memory tests". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 32 (8): 1185–1194. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.1185. hdl:10366/157376. PMID 16938055. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Perfect, Timothy J.; Stark, Louisa-Jayne; Tree, Jeremy J.; Moulin, Christopher J.A.; Ahmed, Lubna; Hutter, Russell (1 October 2004). "Transfer appropriate forgetting: The cue-dependent nature of retrieval-induced forgetting". Journal of Memory and Language. 51 (3): 399–417. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.06.003. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Perfect, Timothy J.; Stark, Louisa-Jayne; Tree, Jeremy J.; Moulin, Christopher J.A.; Ahmed, Lubna; Hutter, Russell (1 October 2004). "Transfer appropriate forgetting: The cue-dependent nature of retrieval-induced forgetting". Journal of Memory and Language. 51 (3): 399–417. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.06.003. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Perfect, Timothy J.; Stark, Louisa-Jayne; Tree, Jeremy J.; Moulin, Christopher J.A.; Ahmed, Lubna; Hutter, Russell (1 October 2004). "Transfer appropriate forgetting: The cue-dependent nature of retrieval-induced forgetting". Journal of Memory and Language. 51 (3): 399–417. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.06.003. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Bäuml, Karl Heinz; Hartinger, Armin (2002). "On the role of item similarity in retrieval-induced forgetting". Memory. 10 (3): 215–224. doi:10.1080/09658210143000362. PMID 11958725. S2CID 31878520. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Perfect, Timothy J.; Stark, Louisa-Jayne; Tree, Jeremy J.; Moulin, Christopher J.A.; Ahmed, Lubna; Hutter, Russell (1 October 2004). "Transfer appropriate forgetting: The cue-dependent nature of retrieval-induced forgetting". Journal of Memory and Language. 51 (3): 399–417. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.06.003. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Perfect, Timothy J.; Stark, Louisa-Jayne; Tree, Jeremy J.; Moulin, Christopher J.A.; Ahmed, Lubna; Hutter, Russell (1 October 2004). "Transfer appropriate forgetting: The cue-dependent nature of retrieval-induced forgetting". Journal of Memory and Language. 51 (3): 399–417. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.06.003. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Aslan, Alp; Bäuml, Karl Heinz; Pastotter, Bernhardt (2007). "No Inhibitory Deficit in Older Adults' Episodic Memory". Psychological Science. 18 (1): 72–78. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01851.x. PMID 17362381. S2CID 30263230. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Jakab, Emőke; Raaijmakers, Jeroen G. W. (2009). "The Role of Item Strength in Retrieval-Induced Forgetting". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 35 (3): 607–617. doi:10.1037/a0015264. PMID 19379039. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Camp, Gino; Pecher, Diane; Schmidt, Henk G. (1 January 2007). "No retrieval-induced forgetting using item-specific independent cues: Evidence against a general inhibitory account". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 33 (5): 950–958. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.5.950. PMID 17723071. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Kuhl, Bruce A.; Dudukovic, Nicole M.; Wagner, Anthony D. (July 2007). "Decreased demands on cognitive control reveal the neural processing benefits of forgetting". Nature Neuroscience. 10 (7): 908–914. doi:10.1038/nn1918. PMID 17558403. S2CID 7711389. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Bäuml, Karl Heinz; Hartinger, Armin (2002). "On the role of item similarity in retrieval-induced forgetting". Memory. 10 (3): 215–224. doi:10.1080/09658210143000362. PMID 11958725. S2CID 31878520. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Storm, Benjamin C.; Bjork, Elizabeth L.; Bjork, Robert A.; Nestojko, John F. (2006). "Is retrieval success a necessary condition for retrieval-induced forgetting?". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 13 (6): 1023–1027. doi:10.3758/bf03213919. PMID 17484429. https://doi.org/10.3758%2Fbf03213919
Anderson, Michael C.; Bjork, Robert A.; Bjork, Elizabeth L. (1994). "Remembering Can Cause Forgetting: Retrieval Dynamics in Long-Term Memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 20 (5): 1063–1087. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.119.3933. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.5.1063. PMID 7931095. /wiki/CiteSeerX_(identifier)
Bajo, M. Teresa; Gómez-Ariza, C.J.; Fernandez, C.; Marful, A. (September 2006). "Retrieval-induced forgetting in perceptually driven memory tests". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 32 (8): 1185–1194. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.1185. hdl:10366/157376. PMID 16938055. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Román, Patricia; Soriano, M.F.; Gómez-Ariza, C.J.; Bajo, M.T. (2009). "Retrieval-induced forgetting and executive control". Psychological Science. 20 (9): 1053–1058. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02415.x. PMID 19656337. S2CID 6097660. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Spitzer, Bernhard; Bäuml, K. (2007). "Retrieval-induced forgetting in item recognition: Evidence for a reduction in general memory strength". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 83 (5): 863–875. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.5.863. PMID 17723065. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Román, Patricia; Soriano, M.F.; Gómez-Ariza, C.J.; Bajo, M.T. (2009). "Retrieval-induced forgetting and executive control". Psychological Science. 20 (9): 1053–1058. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02415.x. PMID 19656337. S2CID 6097660. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Veling, Harm; Van Knippenberg, A. (204). "Remembering can cause inhibition: Retrieval-induced inhibition as cue independent process". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 30 (2): 315–318. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.2.315. hdl:2066/64639. PMID 14979806. S2CID 1474011. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Esgate, Anthony; Groome, David; Baker, Kevin (2005). An Introduction to Applied Cognitive Psychology. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. p. 28. ISBN 978-1841693187. 978-1841693187
"The Cognitive Process Behind "Retrieval-Induced Forgetting"". Medical News Today. 26 February 2012. Retrieved 8 November 2012. http://www.medicalnewstoday.com/releases/242122.php
Shivde, G.; Anderson, M.C. (2001). "The role of inhibition in meaning selection: Insights from retrieval-induced forgetting". In D.S. Gorfein (ed.). On the Consequences of Meaning Selection: Perspectives on Resolving Lexical Ambiguity. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. pp. 175–190. ISBN 978-1557988799. 978-1557988799
Johnson, S.K.; Anderson, M.C. (2004). "The role of inhibitory control in forgetting semantic knowledge". Psychological Science. 15 (7): 448–453. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00700.x. PMID 15200628. S2CID 16809265. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Bajo, M. Teresa; Gómez-Ariza, C.J.; Fernandez, C.; Marful, A. (September 2006). "Retrieval-induced forgetting in perceptually driven memory tests". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 32 (8): 1185–1194. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.5.1185. hdl:10366/157376. PMID 16938055. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Ciranni, M. A.; Shimamura, A. P. (1999). "Retrieval-induced forgetting in episodic memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 25 (6): 1403–1414. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.6.1403. PMID 10605828. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Gómez-Ariza, C.; Fernandez, A.; Bajo, M.T. (2012). "Incidental retrieval-induced forgetting of location information". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 19 (3): 483–489. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0228-8. hdl:10366/157289. PMID 22351590. https://doi.org/10.3758%2Fs13423-012-0228-8
Anderson, M.C.; Bell, T. (2001). "Forgetting our facts: The role of inhibitory processes in the loss of propositional knowledge". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 130 (3): 544–570. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.544. PMID 11561927. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Shaw, J.S.; Bjork, R.A.; Handal, A. (1995). "Retrieval-induced forgetting in an eyewitness-memory paradigm". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2 (2): 249–253. doi:10.3758/bf03210965. PMID 24203660. https://doi.org/10.3758%2Fbf03210965
MacLeod, M. (2002). "Retrieval-induced forgetting in eyewitness memory: forgetting as a consequence of remembering". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 16 (2): 135–149. doi:10.1002/acp.782. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
MacLeod, M.; Macrae, C.N. (March 2001). "Gone but not forgotten: The transient nature of retrieval-induced forgetting". Psychological Science. 12 (2): 148–152. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00325. PMID 11340924. S2CID 28344412. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Esgate, Anthony; Groome, David; Baker, Kevin (2005). Anthony Esgate (ed.). An Introduction to Applied Cognitive Psychology. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. p. 30. ISBN 978-1841693187. 978-1841693187
Storm, Benjamin C.; Bjork, Elizabeth L.; Bjork, Robert A.; Nestojko, John F. (2006). "Is retrieval success a necessary condition for retrieval-induced forgetting?". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 13 (6): 1023–1027. doi:10.3758/bf03213919. PMID 17484429. https://doi.org/10.3758%2Fbf03213919
Storm, B.C.; Bjork, E.L.; Bjork, R.A.; Nestojko, J.F. (2007). "When intended remembering leads to unintended forgetting". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 60 (7): 909–915. doi:10.1080/17470210701288706. PMID 17616909. S2CID 14758513. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Storm, B.C.; Bjork, E.L.; Bjork, R.A.; Nestojko, J.F. (2007). "When intended remembering leads to unintended forgetting". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 60 (7): 909–915. doi:10.1080/17470210701288706. PMID 17616909. S2CID 14758513. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Storm, B.C.; Nestojko, J.F. (2010). "Successful inhibition, unsuccessful retrieval: Manipulating time and success during retrieval practice". Memory. 18 (2): 99–114. doi:10.1080/09658210903107853. PMID 19657962. S2CID 2913524. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Storm, B.C.; Bjork, E.L.; Bjork, R.A.; Nestojko, J.F. (2007). "When intended remembering leads to unintended forgetting". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 60 (7): 909–915. doi:10.1080/17470210701288706. PMID 17616909. S2CID 14758513. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Bäuml, Karl Heinz; Hartinger, Armin (2002). "On the role of item similarity in retrieval-induced forgetting". Memory. 10 (3): 215–224. doi:10.1080/09658210143000362. PMID 11958725. S2CID 31878520. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Ciranni, M. A.; Shimamura, A. P. (1999). "Retrieval-induced forgetting in episodic memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 25 (6): 1403–1414. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.25.6.1403. PMID 10605828. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Storm, B.C.; White, H.A. (2010). "ADHD and retrieval-induced forgetting: Evidence for a deficit in the inhibitory control of memory". Memory. 18 (3): 265–271. doi:10.1080/09658210903547884. PMID 20209425. S2CID 205665474. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Potts, Rosalind; Law, Robin; Golding, John F.; Groome, David (2012). "The Reliability of Retrieval-Induced Forgetting". European Psychologist. 17 (1): 1–10. doi:10.1027/1016-9040/a000040. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Potts, Rosalind; Law, Robin; Golding, John F.; Groome, David (2012). "The Reliability of Retrieval-Induced Forgetting". European Psychologist. 17 (1): 1–10. doi:10.1027/1016-9040/a000040. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Brown, Adam D.; Kouri, Nicole; Hirst, William (1 January 2012). "Memory's malleability: Its role in shaping collective memory and social identity". Frontiers in Psychology. 3: 257. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00257. PMC 3402138. PMID 22837750. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3402138
Rajaram, S. (2011). "Collaboration both hurts and helps memory". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 20 (2): 76–81. doi:10.1177/0963721411403251. S2CID 19179853. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Coman, A.; Manier, D.; Hirst, W. (2009). "Forgetting the unforgettable through conversation: Socially shared retrieval-induced forgetting of September 11 memories". Psychological Science. 20 (5): 627–633. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02343.x. PMID 19476592. S2CID 5936888. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Coman, A.; Momennejad, I.; Drach, R.; Geana, A. (2016). "Mnemonic convergence in social networks: The emergent properties of cognition at a collective level". PNAS. 113 (29): 8171–8176. Bibcode:2016PNAS..113.8171C. doi:10.1073/pnas.1525569113. PMC 4961177. PMID 27357678. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4961177
Anderson, Michael C.; Bjork, Robert A.; Bjork, Elizabeth L. (1994). "Remembering Can Cause Forgetting: Retrieval Dynamics in Long-Term Memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 20 (5): 1063–1087. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.119.3933. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.5.1063. PMID 7931095. /wiki/CiteSeerX_(identifier)
Mensink, G., G.; Raaijmakers, J.G.W. (1988). "A Model for interference and forgetting". Psychological Review. 95 (4): 434–455. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.95.4.434. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Anderson, Michael C.; Bjork, Robert A.; Bjork, Elizabeth L. (1994). "Remembering Can Cause Forgetting: Retrieval Dynamics in Long-Term Memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 20 (5): 1063–1087. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.119.3933. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.5.1063. PMID 7931095. /wiki/CiteSeerX_(identifier)
Verde, M.F. (2012-02-24). "Retrieval-induced forgetting and Inhibition: A critical view". In Brian H. Ross (ed.). The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Oxford: Academic Press. pp. 47–80. ISBN 978-0-12-394393-4. 978-0-12-394393-4
Malmberg, K. J.; Shiffrin, R.M. (2005). "The "one-shot" hypothesis for context storage". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 31 (2): 322–336. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.31.2.322. PMID 15755248. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Verde, M. F.; Perfect, T.J. (2011). "Retrieval-induced forgetting in recognition is absent under time pressure". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 18 (6): 1166–1171. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0143-4. PMID 21811896. https://doi.org/10.3758%2Fs13423-011-0143-4
Neumann, Ewald; Cherau, Julie F.; Hood, Kristin, L. & Steinnagel, Shaunel, L. (1993). "Does inhibition spread in a manner analogous to spreading activation?". Memory. 1 (2): 81–105. doi:10.1080/09658219308258226. PMID 7584264.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Neumann, Ewald; DeSchepper, Brett G. (1992). "An inhibition-based fan effect: Evidence for an active suppression mechanism in selective attention". Canadian Journal of Psychology. 46 (1): 1–40. doi:10.1037/h0084309. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
"The Cognitive Process Behind "Retrieval-Induced Forgetting"". Medical News Today. 26 February 2012. Retrieved 8 November 2012. http://www.medicalnewstoday.com/releases/242122.php
Storm, B.C.; Levy, B.J. (2012). "A progress report on the inhibitory account of retrieval-induced forgetting". Memory & Cognition. 40 (6): 827–843. doi:10.3758/s13421-012-0211-7. PMID 22552992. https://doi.org/10.3758%2Fs13421-012-0211-7
Anderson, Michael C.; Levy, Benjamin J. (2007). "Theoretical issues in inhibition: Insights from research on human memory.". In Gorfein, David S.; MacLeod, Colin M. (eds.). Inhibition in Cognition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. pp. 81–102. http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/2007-07643-005
Robert A. Bjork (1989). Henry L. Roediger; Fergus I. M. Craik (eds.). Varieties of Memory and Consciousness: Essays in Honour of Endel Tulving. Psychology Press. p. 337. ISBN 978-0898599350. 978-0898599350
Storm, B.C.; Levy, B.J. (2012). "A progress report on the inhibitory account of retrieval-induced forgetting". Memory & Cognition. 40 (6): 827–843. doi:10.3758/s13421-012-0211-7. PMID 22552992. https://doi.org/10.3758%2Fs13421-012-0211-7
Anderson, Michael C.; Levy, Benjamin J. (2007). "Theoretical issues in inhibition: Insights from research on human memory.". In Gorfein, David S.; MacLeod, Colin M. (eds.). Inhibition in Cognition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. pp. 81–102. http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/2007-07643-005
Anderson, Michael C.; Levy, Benjamin J. (2007). "Theoretical issues in inhibition: Insights from research on human memory.". In Gorfein, David S.; MacLeod, Colin M. (eds.). Inhibition in Cognition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. pp. 81–102. http://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/2007-07643-005
Kozlovskiy, S.; Glazkova, A.; Kashirin, V. (June 2023). "Electrical activity of the frontal lobes in retrieval-induced forgetting". International Journal of Psychophysiology. 188: 116. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2023.05.296. S2CID 259593192. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0167876023003768
M. MacLeod, Colin; Michael D. Dodd; Erin D. Sheard; Daryl E. Wilson; Uri Bibi (2003). "In opposition to inhibition". In B. H. Ross (ed.). The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Elsevier. pp. 163–214. ISBN 9780080522753. 9780080522753
Dodd, M.D.; Castel, A.D; Roberts, K.E. (2006). "A strategy disruption component to retrieval-induced forgetting". Memory & Cognition. 34 (1): 102–11. doi:10.3758/bf03193390. PMID 16686110. https://doi.org/10.3758%2Fbf03193390
Jakab, E.; Raaijmakers, J.G.W (2009). "The role of item strength in retrieval-induced forgetting". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 35 (3): 607–617. doi:10.1037/a0015264. PMID 19379039. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)