In general, in the usability evaluation both retrospective and concurrent TAP could be used according to the aims and goals of the study. Nevertheless, when a usability evaluation is carried out with blind people several studies propose to use the retrospective TAP: indeed, using a screen reader and talking about the way of interacting with the computer implies a structural interference between action and verbalization. Undoubtedly, cognitive studies provide a lot of evidence supporting the idea that individuals can listen, verbalize, manipulate, and rescue information in multiple task conditions. As Colin Cherry4 showed, subjects, when listening to two different messages from a single loudspeaker, can separate sounds from background noise, recognize the gender of the speaker, the direction, and the pitch (cocktail party effect). At the same time, subjects that must verbalize the content of a message (attended message) while listening to two different messages simultaneously (attended and unattended message) have a reduced ability to report the content of the attended message, while they are unable to report the content of the unattended message. Moreover, K. Anders Ericsson and Walter Kintsch5 showed that, in a multiple task condition, subjects' ability of rescuing information is not compromised by an interruption of the action flow (as it happens in the concurrent thinking aloud technique), thanks to the “Long Term Working Memory mechanism” of information retrieval (Working Memory section Ericsson and Kintsch). Even if users can listen, recognize, and verbalize multiple messages in a multiple task condition and they can stop and restart actions without losing any information, other cognitive studies underlined that the overlap of activities in a multiple task condition have an effect on the goal achievement: Kemper, Herman and Lian,6 analysing the users' abilities to verbalize actions in a multiple task condition, showed that the fluency of a user's conversation is influenced by the overlap of actions. Adults are likely to continue to talk as they navigate in a complex physical environment. However, the fluency of their conversation is likely to change: Older adults are likely to speak more slowly than they would if resting; Young adults continue to speak just as rapidly while walking as while resting, but they adopt a further set of speech accommodations, reducing sentence length, grammatical complexity, and propositional density. Just by reducing length, complexity, and propositional density adults free up working memory resources. We do not know how and how much the content of verbalizations could be influenced by the strategy of verbalization (i.e. the modification of fluency and the complexity in a multiple task condition). Anyway, we well know that users in the concurrent thinking aloud verbalize the problems in a more accurate and pertinent way (i.e. more focused on the problems directly perceived during the interaction) then in the retrospective one.78 The pertinence is granted to the user by the proximity of action-verbalization-next action; this multiple task proximity compels the subject to apply a strategy of verbalization that reduce the overload of the working memory. However, for blind users this time proximity between action and verbalization is lost: the use of the screen reader, in fact, increase the time for verbalization (i.e. in order to verbalize, blind users must first stop the [screen reader] and then restart it).
PCTA method is composed of two sections, one concurrent and one retrospective:
The first section is a modified concurrent protocol built up according to the three concurrent verbal protocols criteria described by K. Anders Ericsson and Herbert A. Simon:910
The second PCTA section is a retrospective one in which users analyse those problems previously verbalized in a concurrent way. The memory signs, created by users ringing the desk-bell, overcome the limits of classic retrospective analysis; indeed, these signs allow the users to be pertinent and consistent with their concurrent verbalization, thus avoiding the influence of long term memory and perceptual reworking.
Borsci, S., & Federici, S. (2009). "The Partial Concurrent Thinking Aloud: A New Usability Evaluation Technique for Blind Users". In P. L. Emiliani; L. Burzagli; A. Como; F. Gabbanini; A. L. Salminen (eds.). Assistive technology from adapted equipment to inclusive environments. Vol. 25. IOS Press. pp. 421–425.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) /wiki/Template:Cite_book ↩
"ECoNA - Home Page". Archived from the original on 2010-02-10. Retrieved 2010-02-04. https://web.archive.org/web/20100210202829/http://w3.uniroma1.it/econa/ ↩
Federici, S., Borsci, S., & Stamerra, G. (November 2009). "Web usability evaluation with screen reader users: implementation of the partial concurrent thinking aloud technique". Cognitive Processing. 11 (3): 263–72. doi:10.1007/s10339-009-0347-y. PMID 19916036. S2CID 2155123.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) /wiki/Doi_(identifier) ↩
Cherry, E.C. (1953). "Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 25 (5): 975–979. Bibcode:1953ASAJ...25..975C. doi:10.1121/1.1907229. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002A-F750-3. /wiki/Bibcode_(identifier) ↩
Ericsson, K.A., Kintsch, W. (1995). "Long-Term Working Memory". Psychological Review. 102 (2): 211–245. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.2.211. PMID 7740089.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) /wiki/Doi_(identifier) ↩
Kemper, S., Herman, R.E., & Lian, C.H.T. (2003). "The Costs of Doing Two Things at Once for Young and Older Adults: Talking While Walking, Finger Tapping, and Ignoring Speech or Noise". Psychology and Aging. 18 (2): 181–192. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.18.2.181. hdl:1808/8613. PMID 12825768.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) /wiki/Doi_(identifier) ↩
Bowers, V.A & Snyder, H.L. (2003). Concurrent versus retrospective verbal protocols for comparing window usability. Human Factors Society 34th Meeting, 8–12 October 1990 HFES, Santa Monica. pp. 1270–1274. ↩
Van den Haak, M.J. & De Jong, M.D.T. (2003). Exploring Two Methods of Usability Testing: Concurrent versus Retrospective Think-Aloud Protocols. IEEE International Professional Communication Conference Proceedings Piscataway, New Jersey. ↩
Ericsson, K.A., Simon, H.A. (1980). "Verbal reports as data". Psychological Review. 87 (3): 215–251. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.87.3.215.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) /wiki/Doi_(identifier) ↩
Ericsson, K.A., Simon, H.A. (1993). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data (Revised ed.). MIT Press Cambridge.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) /wiki/Template:Cite_book ↩