Chomsky's interest in language started at an early age. When he was twelve, he studied Hebrew grammar under his father. He also studied Arabic in his first year at the University of Pennsylvania. In 1947, he met Zellig Harris, the founder of the college's linguistics department. Harris was an established linguist. He did research in the way laid out by American linguist Leonard Bloomfield. He let Chomsky proofread a copy of his book Methods in Structural Linguistics (1951). This is how Chomsky came to know a formal theory of linguistics. He soon decided to major in the subject.
From there on, Chomsky tried to build a grammar of Hebrew. Such a grammar would generate the phonetic or sound forms of sentences. To this end, he organized Harris's methods in a different way. To describe sentence forms and structures, he came up with a set of recursive rules. These are rules that refer back to themselves. He also found that there were many different ways of presenting the grammar. He tried to develop a method to measure how simple a grammar is. For this, he looked for "generalizations" among the possible sets of grammatical rules. Chomsky completed his undergraduate thesis The Morphophonemics of Modern Hebrew in 1949. He then published a revised and expanded version of it as his master's thesis in 1951.
"We demand for example from the theory of language that it allow to describe correctly and exhaustively not only such given French text, but also all existing French texts, and not only these but also all possible and conceivable French texts."
When this work is done to a satisfactory level, it will also become possible to predict all the grammatical sentences of a given language[original research?]:
"Thanks to the linguistic knowledge thus acquired, we will be able to construct, for the same language, all conceivable or theoretically possible texts."
Hjelmslev also points out that an algorithmic description of a language could generate an infinite number of products from a finite number of primitive elements:[need quotation to verify]
"When we compare the inventories yielded at the various stages of the deduction, their size will usually turn out to decrease as the procedure goes on. If the text is unrestricted, i.e., capable of being prolonged through constant addition of further parts … it will be possible to register an unrestricted number of sentences"
"The fundamental aim in the linguistic analysis of a language L is to separate the grammatical sequences which are the sentences of L from the ungrammatical sequences which are not sentences of L. The grammar of L will thus be a device that generates all of the grammatical sequences of L and none of the ungrammatical ones"
— Noam Chomsky, Chomsky, like Harris and other American linguists, agreed that there is no causal link from semantics to syntax.[need quotation to verify]
How to translate this idea into a scientific statement remained a vexing issue in American linguistics for decades.[need quotation to verify] Harris and Rulon Wells justified analyzing the object as part of the verb phrase per 'economy'; but this term, again, merely suggested the perceived 'easiness' of the practice.[need quotation to verify]
In 1955, Chomsky had a doctorate in linguistics. Even so, he struggled at first to publish his theory and views on language. He offered the manuscript of The Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory (LSLT) for publication. But MIT's Technology Press refused to publish it. He also saw a paper promptly rejected by the academic linguistics journal WORD. So he remained an outsider to the field of linguistics. His reviews and articles at the time were mostly published in non-linguistic journals.
Chomsky then prepared a manuscript of the right size (no longer than 120 pages) that would fit the series. After revising an earlier manuscript, Chomsky sent a final version in the first week of August in 1956 to van Schooneveld. The editor had Chomsky rename the book's title to Syntactic Structures for commercial purposes. The book was also pre-ordered in big numbers by MIT. These gave more incentives to Mouton to publish the book. Mouton finally published Chomsky's monograph titled Syntactic Structures in the second week of February 1957.
The second chapter is titled "The Independence of Grammar". In it, Chomsky states that a language is "a set ... of sentences, each finite in length and constructed out of a finite set of elements". A linguist should separate the "grammatical sequences" or sentences of a language from the "ungrammatical sequences". By a "grammatical" sentence Chomsky means a sentence that is intuitively "acceptable to a native speaker". It is a sentence pronounced with a "normal sentence intonation". It is also "recall[ed] much more quickly" and "learn[ed] much more easily".
Chomsky then analyzes further about the basis of "grammaticality." He shows three ways that do not determine whether a sentence is grammatical or not. First, a grammatical sentence need not be included in a corpus. Secondly, it need not be meaningful. Finally, it does not have to be statistically probable. Chomsky shows all three points using a nonsensical sentence "Colorless green ideas sleep furiously." He writes that the sentence is instinctively "grammatical" to a native English speaker. But it is not included in any known corpus at the time and is neither meaningful nor statistically probable.
Chomsky concludes that "grammar is autonomous and independent of meaning." He adds that "probabilistic models give no particular insight into some of the basic problems of syntactic structure."
British linguist Marcus Tomalin stated that a version of "Colorless green ideas sleep furiously" was suggested decades earlier by Rudolf Carnap. This German philosopher offered in 1934 the pseudo-sentence "Piroten karulieren elatisch". According to American linguist Reese Heitner, Carnap's sentence showed the autonomy of both syntactic and phonological structures.
In the third chapter titled "An Elementary Linguistic Theory", Chomsky tries to determine what sort of device or model gives an adequate account of a given set of "grammatical" sentences. Chomsky hypothesizes that this device has to be finite instead of infinite. He then considers finite state grammar, a communication theoretic model which treats language as a Markov process. Then in the fourth chapter titled "Phrase Structure", he discusses phrase structure grammar, a model based on immediate constituent analysis. In the fifth chapter titled "Limitations of Phrase Structure Description", he claims to show that both these models are inadequate for the purpose of linguistic description. As a solution, he introduces transformational generative grammar (TGG), "a more powerful model ... that might remedy these inadequacies."
Chomsky also borrowed the term "generative" from a previous work of mathematician Emil Post. Post wanted to "mechanically [derive] inferences from an initial axiomatic sentence". Chomsky applied Post's work on logical inference to describe sets of strings (sequence of letters or sounds) of a human language. When he says a finite set of rules "generate" (i.e. "recursively enumerate") the set of potentially infinite number of sentences of a particular human language, he means that they provide an explicit, structural description of those sentences.
In the sixth chapter titled "On the Goals of Linguistic Theory", Chomsky writes that his "fundamental concern" is "the problem of justification of grammars". He draws parallels between the theory of language and theories in physical sciences. He compares a finite corpus of utterances of a particular language to "observations". He likens grammatical rules to "laws" which are stated in terms of "hypothetical constructs" such as phonemes, phrases, etc. According to Chomsky, the criteria for the "justification of grammars" are "external conditions of adequacy", the "condition of generality" and "simplicity". To choose the best possible grammar for a given corpus of a given language, Chomsky shows his preference for the "evaluation procedure" (which uses the aforementioned criteria). He rejects the "discovery procedure" (employed in structural linguistics and supposed to automatically and mechanically produce the correct grammar of a language from a corpus). He also dismisses the "decision procedure" (supposed to automatically choose the best grammar for a language from a set of competing grammars). Chomsky thus shows preference for "explanatory depth" with some "empirical inadequacies" over the pursuit of very detailed empirical coverage of all data.
In the seventh chapter titled "Some Transformations in English", Chomsky strictly applies his just-proposed transformation-based approach on some aspects of English. He treats at length the formation of English negative passive sentences, yes-no and wh- interrogative sentences, etc. He claims in the end that transformational analysis can describe "a wide variety of ... distinct phenomena" in English grammar in a "simple", "natural" and "orderly" way.
In the eighth chapter titled "The explanatory power of linguistic theory", Chomsky writes a linguistic theory cannot content itself by just generating valid grammatical sentences. It also has to account for other structural phenomena at different levels of linguistic representation. At a certain linguistic level, there can be two items which can be understood having different meanings but they are structurally indistinguishable within that level. This is called a "constructional homonymity" [sic]. The relevant ambiguity can be resolved by establishing a higher level of linguistic analysis. At this higher level, the two items can be clearly shown having two different structural interpretations. In this way, constructional homonymities at the phonemic level can be resolved by establishing the level of morphology, and so forth. One of the motivations of establishing a distinct, higher level of linguistic analysis is, then, to explain the structural ambiguity due to the constructional homonymities at a lower level. On the other hand, each linguistic level also captures some structural similarities within the level that are not explained in lower levels. Chomsky uses this argument as well to motivate the establishment of distinct levels of linguistic analysis.
Chomsky then shows that a grammar which analyzes sentences up to the phrase structure level contains many constructional homonymities at the phrase structure level where the resulting ambiguities need to be explained at a higher level. Then he shows how his newly invented "transformational level" can naturally and successfully function as that higher level. He further claims that any phrase structure grammar which cannot explain these ambiguities as successfully as transformational grammar does must be considered "inadequate".
In the ninth chapter titled "Syntax and Semantics", Chomsky reminds that his analysis so far has been "completely formal and non-semantic." He then offers many counterexamples to refute some common linguistic assertions about grammar's reliance on meaning. He concludes that the correspondence between meaning and grammatical form is "imperfect", "inexact" and "vague." Consequently, it is "relatively useless" to use meaning "as a basis for grammatical description". To support his point, Chomsky considers a similar relation between semantics and phonology. He shows that in order to build a theory of phonemic distinction based on meaning would entail "complex", "exhaustive" and "laborious investigation" of an "immense", "vast corpus". By contrast, phonemic distinctness can be easily explained in a "straightforward" way and in "completely non-semantic terms" with the help of "pair tests". Chomsky also claims that a strictly formal, non-semantic framework of syntactic theory might ultimately be useful to support a parallel independent semantic theory.
In particular, Chomsky's analysis of the complex English auxiliary verb system in Syntactic Structures had great rhetorical effect. It combined simple phrase structure rules with a simple transformational rule. This treatment was based entirely on formal simplicity. Various linguists have described it as "beautiful", "powerful", "elegant", "revealing", "insightful", "beguiling" and "ingenious". According to American linguist Frederick Newmeyer, this particular analysis won many "supporters for Chomsky" and "immediately led to some linguists' proposing generative-transformational analysis of particular phenomena". According to British linguist E. Keith Brown, "the elegance and insightfulness of this account was instantly recognized, and this was an important factor in ensuring the initial success of the transformational way of looking at syntax." American linguist Mark Aronoff wrote that this "beautiful analysis and description of some very striking facts was the rhetorical weapon that drove the acceptance of [Chomsky's] theory". He added that in Chomsky's treatment of English verbs, "the convergence of theory and analysis provide a description of facts so convincing that it changed the entire field".
Raymond Oenbring, a doctorate in the rhetoric of science, thinks that Chomsky "overstates the novelty" of transformational rules. He "seems to take all the credit for them" even though a version of them had already been introduced by Zellig Harris in a previous work. He writes that Chomsky himself was "cautious" to "display deference" to prevailing linguistic research. His enthusiastic followers such as Lees were, by contrast, much more "confrontational". They sought to drive a "rhetorical wedge" between Chomsky's work and that of post-Bloomfieldians (i.e. American linguists in the 1940s et 1950s), arguing that the latter does not qualify as linguistic "science".
In 2011, a group of French neuroscientists conducted research to verify if actual brain mechanisms worked in the way that Chomsky suggested in Syntactic Structures. The results suggested that specific regions of the brain handle syntactic information in an abstract way. These are independent from other brain regions that handle semantic information. Moreover, the brain analyzes not just mere strings of words, but hierarchical structures of constituents. These observations validated the theoretical claims of Chomsky in Syntactic Structures.
Cook 2007 - Cook, Vivian (2007), "Chomsky's Syntactic Structures fifty years on", International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 17 (1): 120–131, doi:10.1111/j.1473-4192.2007.00137.x, archived from the original on 2013-01-05 https://archive.today/20130105113213/http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/118505208/abstract
Grossman, Lev (17 August 2016). "All-TIME 100 Nonfiction Books : Syntactic Structures". Time. Retrieved 14 October 2016. https://entertainment.time.com/2011/08/30/all-time-100-best-nonfiction-books/slide/syntactic-structures-by-noam-chomsky/
Chomsky 1957, p. 15 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 17 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
From Chomsky 1957, p. 103:"...such semantic notions as reference, significance, and synonymity played no role in the discussion." - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky is quoted in Riemsdijk & Huybregts 1982, p. 63 saying: "It [Syntactic Structures] was course notes for an undergraduate course at MIT. Van Schooneveld [a Dutch linguist who was associated with Mouton] showed up here once and took a look at some of my course notes from the undergraduate course I was teaching and said I ought to publish it." In (Dillinger & Palácio 1997, pp. 162–163), Chomsky recounted: "At the time Mouton was publishing just about anything, so they decided they'd publish it along with a thousand other worthless things that were coming out. That's the story of Syntactic Structures: course notes for undergraduate science students published by accident in Europe." The publication of Syntactic structures is also discussed in Noordegraaf 2001 and van Schooneveld 2001. - Riemsdijk, Henk C. van.; Huybregts, Riny (1982), The Generative Enterprise: A Discussion, Dordrecht, Holland: Foris Publications https://books.google.com/books?id=eJ44uAAACAAJ
Seuren, Pieter A. M. (1998). Western linguistics: An historical introduction. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 250–251. ISBN 0-631-20891-7. 0-631-20891-7
From Chomsky 1957, p. 102: "In §§3-7 we outlined the development of some fundamental linguistic concepts in purely formal terms." - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 44 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 13 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Here, "generate" means giving a clear structural description of each sentence. In Chomsky 1965, p. 9, Chomsky writes that "When we speak of a grammar as generating a sentence with a certain structural description, we mean simply that the grammar assigns this structural description to the sentence." - Chomsky, Noam (1965), Aspects of the Theory of Syntax, Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press, ISBN 978-0-262-52740-8
Seuren, Pieter A. M. (1998). Western linguistics: An historical introduction. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 250–251. ISBN 0-631-20891-7. 0-631-20891-7
Chomsky 1957, p. 49 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 85 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
According to Steinberg, Hiroshi & Aline 2013, p. 371: "[Chomsky's generative system of rules] was more powerful that anything ... psycholinguists had heretofore had at their disposal. [It] was of special interest to these theorists. Many psychologists were quick to attribute generative systems to the minds of speakers and quick to abandon ... Behaviorism." - Steinberg, Danny D.; Hiroshi, Nagata; Aline, David P. (2013), Psycholinguistics: Language, Mind and World, Routledge, ISBN 9781317900566 https://books.google.com/books?id=EfK1AQAAQBAJ
Lightfoot, David W. (2002). "Introduction to the second edition of Syntactic Structures by Noam Chomsky". In Lightfoot, David W. (ed.). Syntactic Structures (second ed.). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. v–xviii. ISBN 3110172798. Retrieved 2020-02-26. 3110172798
According to Joseph 2002, p. [page needed], Hjelmslev and other European linguists, in contrast, had considered the generative calculus as perfectly non-psychological. See also Hjelmslev 1969 - Joseph, John E. (2002). From Whitney to Chomsky: Essays in the History of American Linguistics. John Benjamins. ISBN 9789027275370.
Hjelmslev, Louis (1969) [First published 1943]. Prolegomena to a Theory of Language. University of Wisconsin Press. ISBN 0299024709. 0299024709
Pullum 2011 writes: "[Chomsky] was at the time an unknown 28-year-old who taught language classes at MIT" - Pullum, Geoffrey K. (2011), "On the Mathematical Foundations of Syntactic Structures" (PDF), Journal of Logic, Language and Information, 20 (3): 277–296, doi:10.1007/s10849-011-9139-8, hdl:20.500.11820/3bfafa09-b7f2-4249-b2d3-714227f2ddb8, S2CID 26842058 http://www.lel.ed.ac.uk/~gpullum/onSyntacticStructures.pdf
"The Cognitive Science Millennium Project". 2004-08-21. Archived from the original on 2004-08-21. Retrieved 2019-12-31. https://web.archive.org/web/20040821111702/http://www.cogsci.umn.edu/OLD/calendar/past_events/millennium/final.html
See the "Reception" section of this article.
Lightfoot, David W. (2002). "Introduction to the second edition of Syntactic Structures by Noam Chomsky". In Lightfoot, David W. (ed.). Syntactic Structures (second ed.). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. v–xviii. ISBN 3110172798. Retrieved 2020-02-26. 3110172798
Anthropology, Radical (2008). "Human nature and the origins of language" (PDF). Radical Anthropology (2). Archived from the original (PDF) on 2019-12-07. Retrieved 2020-02-25. https://web.archive.org/web/20191207235646/http://radicalanthropologygroup.org/sites/default/files/journal/journal_02.pdf
Johnson, Steven (2002). "Sociobiology and you". The Nation (November 18). Retrieved 2020-02-25. https://www.thenation.com/article/archive/sociobiology-and-you/
See the "Criticisms" section of this article.
Specifically, Chomsky read David Kimhi's Hebrew Grammar (Mikhlol) (1952), an annotated study of a 13th century Hebrew grammar. It was written by his father, William Chomsky, one of the leading Hebrew scholars at the time. See Barsky 1997, p. 10 /wiki/William_Chomsky
For its similarity to Hebrew. See Barsky 1997, p. 47 and "Noam Chomsky interviewed by David Samuels". Chomsky.info. Retrieved 16 November 2016. - Barsky, Robert (1997), Noam Chomsky: A life of Dissent, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press, ISBN 978-0-262-52255-7, archived from the original on 2006-09-06 https://web.archive.org/web/20060906235759/http://cognet.mit.edu/library/books/chomsky/chomsky/
Barsky 1997, p. 48 - Barsky, Robert (1997), Noam Chomsky: A life of Dissent, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press, ISBN 978-0-262-52255-7, archived from the original on 2006-09-06 https://web.archive.org/web/20060906235759/http://cognet.mit.edu/library/books/chomsky/chomsky/
In the 1947 preface of Harris 1951, Zellig Harris writes that “N. Chomsky has given
much-needed assistance with the manuscript." - Harris, Zellig (1951), Methods in Structural Linguistics, Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Barsky 1997, pp. 49–50 - Barsky, Robert (1997), Noam Chomsky: A life of Dissent, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press, ISBN 978-0-262-52255-7, archived from the original on 2006-09-06 https://web.archive.org/web/20060906235759/http://cognet.mit.edu/library/books/chomsky/chomsky/
In his preface to Chomsky 1975, Chomsky writes that “My introduction to the field of linguistics was in 1947, when Zellig Harris gave me the proofs of his 'Methods in Structural Linguistics' to read." - Chomsky, Noam (1975), The Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory, New York: Plenum, ISBN 978-0-306-30760-7
Chomsky 1975, p. 33 and Thomas 2012, p. 250 - Chomsky, Noam (1975), The Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory, New York: Plenum, ISBN 978-0-306-30760-7
Especially Goodman's work on constructional systems and on the inadequacy of inductive approaches. See Chomsky 1975, p. 33. Goldsmith & Huck 1995, p. 24 writes: "Chomsky has said that he was convinced from his days as a student of Goodman's that there is no inductive learning." /wiki/Constructional_system
Chomsky 1975, p. 33 writes: "Quine's critiques of logical empiricism also gave some reason to believe that [a non-taxonomic approach to linguistic theory] might be a plausible one." - Chomsky, Noam (1975), The Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory, New York: Plenum, ISBN 978-0-306-30760-7
Otero 1994 states that among non-American philosophers, it was only Rudolf Carnap whom Chomsky read as a student (p. 3) - Otero, Carlos Peregrín (1994), Noam Chomsky: Critical Assessments, vol. 2, London and New York: Routledge Press
Tomalin 2003 writes that "It is well known that Carnap's post-Aufbau work (especially Logische Syntax der Sprache) influenced Chomsky directly to some extent." - Tomalin, Marcus (2003), "Goodman, Quine, and Chomsky: from a grammatical point of view", Lingua, 113 (12): 1223–1253, CiteSeerX 10.1.1.136.6985, doi:10.1016/s0024-3841(03)00017-2 https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.136.6985
Quine 1951 - Quine, Willard Van Orman (1951), "Two Dogmas of Empiricism", Philosophical Review, 60 (1): 20–43, doi:10.2307/2181906, JSTOR 2181906 https://doi.org/10.2307%2F2181906
Carnap 1934 - Carnap, Rudolf (1934), Logische Syntax der Sprache, Wien (Vienna): Julius Springer, ISBN 978-3-662-23330-6 https://books.google.com/books?id=viaqBgAAQBAJ
Joseph, Love & Taylor 2001, p. 125 states: "The most significant discontinuity [between Harris's Methods and Chomsky's Syntactic Structures] is Chomsky's inversion of Harris's analytic procedures." - Joseph, John E.; Love, Nigel; Taylor, Talbot J. (2001), Landmarks in Linguistic Thought II: The Western Tradition in the Twentieth Century, History of Linguistic Thought, London and New York: Routledge, ISBN 9780415063968 https://books.google.com/books?id=biWz1o-KyGoC
Tomalin 2006, p. 116 writes: "[Echoing] Goodman's pro-simplicity arguments ... the task of creating ... a simplicity measure is precisely the one Chomsky sets for himself in Chapter 4 of LSLT." - Tomalin, Marcus (2006), Linguistics and the Formal Sciences: The Origins of Generative Grammar, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, ISBN 9780511486340
Chomsky 1951, p. 5 states: "We want the reduction of the number of elements and statements, any generalizations ... to increase the total simplicity of the grammar" - Chomsky, Noam (1951), Morphophonemics of Modern Hebrew (M.A. thesis), University of Pennsylvania
Graffi 2001, p. 331 - Graffi, Giorgio (2001), 200 Years of Syntax: A Critical Survey, Studies in the History of the Language Sciences, John Benjamins Publishing, ISBN 978-1588110527 https://books.google.com/books?id=mydolrE-PPkC
Before Chomsky, Israeli mathematician and linguist Yehoshua Bar-Hillel had already shown in Bar-Hillel 1953 that formal languages and methods used in symbolic logic can be adapted to analyze human languages. /wiki/Yehoshua_Bar-Hillel
McGilvray 2005, p. 117 - McGilvray, James (2005), The Cambridge Companion to Chomsky, Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press, ISBN 9780521784313 https://books.google.com/books?id=I6CZ6wpNKeEC
Chomsky 1953 - Chomsky, Noam (1953), "Systems of Syntactic Analysis", Journal of Symbolic Logic, 18 (3): 242–256, doi:10.2307/2267409, JSTOR 2267409, S2CID 11796020 https://doi.org/10.2307%2F2267409
Barsky 1997, p. 83 - Barsky, Robert (1997), Noam Chomsky: A life of Dissent, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press, ISBN 978-0-262-52255-7, archived from the original on 2006-09-06 https://web.archive.org/web/20060906235759/http://cognet.mit.edu/library/books/chomsky/chomsky/
Barsky 1997, p. 86 - Barsky, Robert (1997), Noam Chomsky: A life of Dissent, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press, ISBN 978-0-262-52255-7, archived from the original on 2006-09-06 https://web.archive.org/web/20060906235759/http://cognet.mit.edu/library/books/chomsky/chomsky/
Sampson 2001, p. 152 - Sampson, Geoffrey (2001), Empirical Linguistics, London and New York: Continuum International http://www.grsampson.net/BEmpLj.html
Seuren, Pieter A. M. (1998). Western linguistics: An historical introduction. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 250–251. ISBN 0-631-20891-7. 0-631-20891-7
Seuren, Pieter A. M. (1998). Western linguistics: An historical introduction. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 250–251. ISBN 0-631-20891-7. 0-631-20891-7
Garvin, Paul J. (1954). "Review of Prolegomena to a Theory of Language by Louis Hjelmslev, translated by Francis J. Whitfield". Language. 30 (1): 69–66. doi:10.2307/410221. JSTOR 410221. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Hjelmslev, Louis (1971) [1943]. Prolégomènes à une théorie du langage. Paris: Les éditions de minuit. p. 27. ISBN 2707301345. Nous exigeons par exemple de la théorie du langage qu'elle permettre de décrire non contradictoirement et exhaustivement non seulement tel texte français donné, mais aussi tous les textes français existant, et non seulement ceux-ci mais encore tous les textes français possibles et concevables 2707301345
Hjelmslev, Louis (1971) [1943]. Prolégomènes à une théorie du langage. Paris: Les éditions de minuit. p. 27. ISBN 2707301345. Grâce aux connaissances linguistiques ainsi acquises, nous pourrons construire, pour une même langue, tous les textes concevables ou théoriquement possibles. 2707301345
Seuren, Pieter A. M. (1998). Western linguistics: An historical introduction. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 250–251. ISBN 0-631-20891-7. 0-631-20891-7
Hjelmslev, Louis (1969) [First published 1943]. Prolegomena to a Theory of Language. University of Wisconsin Press. ISBN 0299024709. 0299024709
Seuren, Pieter A. M. (1998). Western linguistics: An historical introduction. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 250–251. ISBN 0-631-20891-7. 0-631-20891-7
Chomsky 1957, pp. 19, 24 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Seuren, Pieter A. M. (1998). Western linguistics: An historical introduction. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 250–251. ISBN 0-631-20891-7. 0-631-20891-7
Seuren, Pieter A. M. (1998). Western linguistics: An historical introduction. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 250–251. ISBN 0-631-20891-7. 0-631-20891-7
Seuren, Pieter A. M. (1998). Western linguistics: An historical introduction. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 250–251. ISBN 0-631-20891-7. 0-631-20891-7
Wells, Rulon S. (1947). "Immediate constituents". Language. 23 (2): 81–117. doi:10.2307/410382. JSTOR 410382. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)
Seuren, Pieter A. M. (1998). Western linguistics: An historical introduction. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 250–251. ISBN 0-631-20891-7. 0-631-20891-7
Hjelmslev, Louis (1969) [First published 1943]. Prolegomena to a Theory of Language. University of Wisconsin Press. ISBN 0299024709. 0299024709
Lightfoot, David W. (2002). "Introduction to the second edition of Syntactic Structures by Noam Chomsky". In Lightfoot, David W. (ed.). Syntactic Structures (second ed.). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. v–xviii. ISBN 3110172798. Retrieved 2020-02-26. 3110172798
Berwick, Robert C.; Chomsky, Noam (2015). Why Only Us: Language and Evolution. MIT Press. ISBN 9780262034241. 9780262034241
Sklar 1968, p. 216 - Sklar, Robert (9 September 1968), "Chomsky's Revolution in Linguistics", The Nation: 213–217
Chomsky writes in Chomsky 1979, pp. 131–132: "As for the reception accorded to LSLT [the Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory], there is little to say. I've already told you that I did not have the impression the reaction on the part of linguists was surprising. I offered LSLT to the MIT-Press – who refused it. Quite rightly, I think, because at that time the situation was very unfavourable for a general book on that subject, especially one by an unknown author. I also submitted a technical article on simplicity and explanation to the journal Word, at the suggestion of Roman Jakobson, but it was rejected virtually by return mail. So I had little hope of seeing any of this work published, at least in a linguistic journal." - Chomsky, Noam (1979), Language and Responsibility, New York: Pantheon, ISBN 978-0-85527-535-8
Barsky 1997, pp. 81–82 - Barsky, Robert (1997), Noam Chomsky: A life of Dissent, Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press, ISBN 978-0-262-52255-7, archived from the original on 2006-09-06 https://web.archive.org/web/20060906235759/http://cognet.mit.edu/library/books/chomsky/chomsky/
In particular, Chomsky wrote an academic paper in 1956 titled Three Models for the Description of Language published in the technological journal IRE Transactions on Information Theory (Chomsky 1956). It foreshadows many of the concepts presented in Syntactic Structures. - Chomsky, Noam (1956), "Three models for the description of language" (PDF), IRE Transactions on Information Theory, 2 (3): 113–124, doi:10.1109/TIT.1956.1056813, S2CID 19519474 https://chomsky.info/wp-content/uploads/195609-.pdf
Hamans 2014 - Hamans, Camiel (2014), "The coming about of Syntactic Structures", Beiträge zur Geschichte der Sprachwissenschaft, 24 (1): 133–156 http://dare.uva.nl/document/2/162510
Hinrichs 2001, p. 5 - Hinrichs, Jan Paul (2001), The C. H. van Schooneveld Collection in Leiden University Library. Editorial correspondence and documents relating to Mouton & Co., The Hague, and other papers in the fields of Slavistics and linguistics, Leiden: Leiden University Library, ISBN 978-90-74204-10-1
The series's editor van Schooneveld is quoted thus in Hinrichs 2001, pp. 5–6: "I had originally conceived of the Janua as a series of small monographs of the size of a large article, too interesting to get drowned in a periodical amongst other contributions and to be lost to oblivion by the current of time." - Hinrichs, Jan Paul (2001), The C. H. van Schooneveld Collection in Leiden University Library. Editorial correspondence and documents relating to Mouton & Co., The Hague, and other papers in the fields of Slavistics and linguistics, Leiden: Leiden University Library, ISBN 978-90-74204-10-1
Jakobson & Halle 1956 - Jakobson, Romam; Halle, Morris (1956), Fundamentals of Language, The Hague: Mouton, ISBN 978-1-178-71814-0
Chomsky, Halle & Lukoff 1956 - Chomsky, Noam; Halle, Morris; Lukoff, Fred (1956), "On Accent and Juncture in English", in M. Halle; H.G. Lunt; H. McLean; C.H. van Schooneveld (eds.), For Roman Jakobson: Essays on the occasion of his sixtieth birthday, The Hague: Mouton & Co., pp. 65–80, ISBN 9780598664198 https://books.google.com/books?id=tPpYAAAAMAAJ
Hinrichs 2001, p. 2 - Hinrichs, Jan Paul (2001), The C. H. van Schooneveld Collection in Leiden University Library. Editorial correspondence and documents relating to Mouton & Co., The Hague, and other papers in the fields of Slavistics and linguistics, Leiden: Leiden University Library, ISBN 978-90-74204-10-1
Hamans 2014 - Hamans, Camiel (2014), "The coming about of Syntactic Structures", Beiträge zur Geschichte der Sprachwissenschaft, 24 (1): 133–156 http://dare.uva.nl/document/2/162510
Chomsky is quoted in Riemsdijk & Huybregts 1982, p. 63 saying: "It [Syntactic Structures] was course notes for an undergraduate course at MIT. Van Schooneveld [a Dutch linguist who was associated with Mouton] showed up here once and took a look at some of my course notes from the undergraduate course I was teaching and said I ought to publish it." In (Dillinger & Palácio 1997, pp. 162–163), Chomsky recounted: "At the time Mouton was publishing just about anything, so they decided they'd publish it along with a thousand other worthless things that were coming out. That's the story of Syntactic Structures: course notes for undergraduate science students published by accident in Europe." The publication of Syntactic structures is also discussed in Noordegraaf 2001 and van Schooneveld 2001. - Riemsdijk, Henk C. van.; Huybregts, Riny (1982), The Generative Enterprise: A Discussion, Dordrecht, Holland: Foris Publications https://books.google.com/books?id=eJ44uAAACAAJ
According to Hinrichs 2001, p. 7, Peter de Ridder, the managing director of Mouton, wrote to van Schooneveld that "new titles in the series [should be] no bigger than about 120 pages." - Hinrichs, Jan Paul (2001), The C. H. van Schooneveld Collection in Leiden University Library. Editorial correspondence and documents relating to Mouton & Co., The Hague, and other papers in the fields of Slavistics and linguistics, Leiden: Leiden University Library, ISBN 978-90-74204-10-1
A scan of Chomsky's own typewritten letter dated 5 August 1956 to Mouton editor Cornelis van Schooneveld can be found in Hamans 2014. This letter accompanied the final version of the manuscript. - Hamans, Camiel (2014), "The coming about of Syntactic Structures", Beiträge zur Geschichte der Sprachwissenschaft, 24 (1): 133–156 http://dare.uva.nl/document/2/162510
Hinrichs 2001, p. 7 mentions De Ridder writing to van Schooneveld that "I am convinced that the book will sell well with this title." - Hinrichs, Jan Paul (2001), The C. H. van Schooneveld Collection in Leiden University Library. Editorial correspondence and documents relating to Mouton & Co., The Hague, and other papers in the fields of Slavistics and linguistics, Leiden: Leiden University Library, ISBN 978-90-74204-10-1
Oenbring 2009 remarks that Lees's review was "hyperbolic", his language "loaded" and Harris 1993 refers to Lees as "Chomsky's Huxley", referring to the proselytizing "bulldog" role played by Thomas Henry Huxley in defense of Charles Darwin's theories on evolution. Voegelin 1958 considers Lees to be "Chomsky's explicator". Chomsky himself considers Lees's review "provocative." (Chomsky 1975, p. 3) - Oenbring, Raymond (2009), Scientific rhetoric and disciplinary identity: A critical rhetorical history of generative grammar (Ph.D. thesis), University of Washington
Lees 1957 - Lees, Robert (1957), "Review of Syntactic Structures" (PDF), Language, 33 (3): 375–408, doi:10.2307/411160, JSTOR 411160, archived from the original (PDF) on 2013-11-26, retrieved 2009-09-17. https://web.archive.org/web/20131126161815/http://lingo.stanford.edu/sag/papers/lees-rev.pdf
Thorne 1965 remarked that "a revolution of the kind Kuhn describes has recently taken place in linguistics – dating from the publication of Chomsky's Syntactic Structures in 1957". According to Sklar 1968: "What has happened in linguistics since Chomsky appeared on the scene almost perfectly fits Kuhn's description of how a scientific revolution works." Searle 1972 writes that "[Chomsky's] revolution followed fairly closely the general pattern described in Thomas Kuhn's The Structure of Scientific Revolutions". - Thorne, James Peter (1965), "Review of P. Postal, Constituent Structure", Journal of Linguistics, 1: 73–6, doi:10.1017/s0022226700001055, S2CID 144201727 https://doi.org/10.1017%2Fs0022226700001055
Koerner 2002 - Koerner, E. F. K. (2002), Towards a history of American linguistics, Routledge Studies in the History of Linguistics 5, London & New York: Routledge Taylor & Francis Group, ISBN 9781134495085 https://books.google.com/books?id=3JWCAgAAQBAJ
Kibbee 2010 - Douglas A. Kibbee, ed. (2010), Chomskyan (R)evolutions, Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company
Newmeyer 1986 - Newmeyer, Frederick J. (1986), "Has There Been a 'Chomskyan Revolution' in Linguistics?", Language, 62 (1): 1–18, doi:10.2307/415597, JSTOR 415597 https://doi.org/10.2307%2F415597
Newmeyer 1996 - Newmeyer, Frederick J. (1996), Generative Linguistics: A Historical Perspective, Routledge history of linguistic thought series, Routledge, ISBN 978-0-415-11553-7 https://books.google.com/books?id=MB8i7owaaNYC
Searle 2002 - Searle, John R. (2002), "End of the Revolution", The New York Review of Books, 49 (3): 33–6 http://www.nybooks.com/articles/2002/02/28/end-of-the-revolution/
Chomsky 1975 - Chomsky, Noam (1975), The Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory, New York: Plenum, ISBN 978-0-306-30760-7
Hinrichs 2001, p. 153 - Hinrichs, Jan Paul (2001), The C. H. van Schooneveld Collection in Leiden University Library. Editorial correspondence and documents relating to Mouton & Co., The Hague, and other papers in the fields of Slavistics and linguistics, Leiden: Leiden University Library, ISBN 978-90-74204-10-1
Chomsky 1962 - Chomsky, Noam (1962), "Синтакси́ческие структу́ры (Sintaksychyeskiye Struktury)", in Vladimir Andreevich Zveginchev (ed.), Novoe v lingvistike (in Russian), vol. 2, translated by Babisky, Konstantin Ivanovich, Moscow: Izd-vo Inostr. Literatury, pp. 412–527
Chomsky 1963 - Chomsky, Noam (1963), 文法の構造 (Bunpō no kōzō) (in Japanese), translated by Isamu, Yasuo, Tokyo: Kenkyusha
Chomsky 1969. - Chomsky, Noam (1969), Structures Syntaxiques (in French), translated by Braudeau, Michel, Paris: Éditions du Seuil
Chomsky 1973a. - Chomsky, Noam (1973a), Strukturen der Syntax (in German), translated by Lange, Klaus-Peter, The Hague: Mouton
Chomsky 1966b. - Chomsky, Noam (1966b), 변형생성문법의_이론 (Byeonhyeongsaengseongmunbeobui_iron) (in Korean), translated by Lee, Seung-Hwan; Lee, Hei-Sook, Seoul: Pan-Korea Book Corporation
Chomsky 1974. - Chomsky, Noam (1974), Estructuras sintácticas (in Spanish), translated by Otero, Carlos Peregrín, Mexico City, Mexico: Siglo XXI, ISBN 978-968-23-0075-2 https://books.google.com/books?id=6tUv4IzUWtwC
Chomsky 1970. - Chomsky, Noam (1970), Le strutture della sintassi, Universale Laterza (in Italian), vol. 129, translated by Antinucci, Francesco, Bari: Laterza
Chomsky 1966a. - Chomsky, Noam (1966a), Syntaktické struktury : logický základ teorie jazyka, O pojmu "gramatické pravidlo." (in Czech), translated by Hlavsa, Zdeněk; Benešová, Eva; Daneš, František, Prague: Academia
Bugarski 1972. - Bugarski, Ranko (1972), Gramatica i um (in Serbo-Croatian), Belgrade: Nolit
Chomsky 1973b. - Chomsky, Noam (1973b), Syntaktiska Strukturer (in Swedish), translated by Löfqvist, Anders; Wigforss, Eva, Lund: Gleerup
Chomsky 1957, Preface - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, pp. 5–6 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 13 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 13 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 16 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 15 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 17 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Tomalin 2002 - Tomalin, Marcus (2002), "The formal origins of syntactic theory", Lingua, 112 (10): 827–848, doi:10.1016/S0024-3841(02)00049-9 https://doi.org/10.1016%2FS0024-3841%2802%2900049-9
Rebuschi 2001, p. 2014 - Rebuschi, Georges (2001), "La grammaire generative du milieu des annees 70 au milieu des annees 90: du modele Standard etendu aux debuts du programme minimaliste", in S. Auroux; et al. (eds.), History of the Language Sciences, Walter De Gruyter, pp. 2084–2110
Carnap 1934, p. 2 - Carnap, Rudolf (1934), Logische Syntax der Sprache, Wien (Vienna): Julius Springer, ISBN 978-3-662-23330-6 https://books.google.com/books?id=viaqBgAAQBAJ
According to Heitner 2005: "[Carnap's sentence] actually does the double duty of demonstrating the "autonomy" of syntactic and phonological structure, an indication that not only can sentences be recognized as syntactically well-formed, but individual words can also be recognized as phonologically well-formed independent of semantics." - Heitner, R. M. (2005), "An odd couple: Chomsky and Quine on the phoneme", Language Sciences, 27: 1–30, doi:10.1016/j.langsci.2003.11.006 https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.langsci.2003.11.006
Chomsky 1957, p. 18 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Specifically, the model proposed in Shannon & Weaver 1949 - Shannon, Claude E.; Weaver, Warren (1949), The Mathematical Theory of Communication, Urbana: University of Illinois Press, hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002C-4314-2, ISBN 978-0-252-72548-7 https://hdl.handle.net/11858%2F00-001M-0000-002C-4314-2
Chomsky 1957, pp. 19–21 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, pp. 26–33 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 49 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 45 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 44 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 46 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 45 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 46 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, pp. 38–40 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Collins 2008, pp. 66–67 - Collins, John (2008), Chomsky: A Guide for the Perplexed, Guides for the Perplexed, London and New York: Bloomsbury Academic, ISBN 978-0-8264-8663-9
In Chomsky 1959, Chomsky writes that he was "following a familiar technical use of the term "generate," cf. Post 1944". In Chomsky 1965, p. 9, Chomksy justifies his choice of the term "generate", writing that "the term 'generate' is familiar in the sense intended here in logic, particularly in Post's theory of combinatorial systems. Furthermore, 'generate' seems to be the most appropriate translation for Humboldt's term erzeugen, which he frequently uses, it seems, in essentially the sense here intended. Since this use of the term 'generate' is well established both in logic and in the tradition of linguistic theory." - Chomsky, Noam (1959), "A Review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior", Language, 35 (1): 26–58, doi:10.2307/411334, JSTOR 411334 http://cogprints.org/1148/1/chomsky.htm
Post 1943, Post 1944 and Pullum & Scholz 2001 - Post, Emil Leon (1943), "Formal Reductions of the General Combinatorial Decision Problem", American Journal of Mathematics, 65 (2): 197–215, doi:10.2307/2371809, JSTOR 2371809 https://doi.org/10.2307%2F2371809
Pullum & Scholz 2001 - Pullum, Geoffrey K.; Scholz, Barbara C. (2001), "On the distinction between model-theoretic and generative-enumerative syntactic frameworks", in Philippe de Groote; Glyn Morrill; Christian Retore (eds.), Logical Aspects of Computational Linguistics: 4th International Conference, Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence, Berlin: Springer Verlag, pp. 17–43
In Chomsky 1965, p. 8, Chomsky writes that "by a generative grammar I mean simply a system of rules that in some explicit and well-defined way assigns structural descriptions to sentences." - Chomsky, Noam (1965), Aspects of the Theory of Syntax, Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press, ISBN 978-0-262-52740-8
Chomsky 1957, p. 49 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 49 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 55 writes: "Our main point is that a linguistic theory should not be identified with a manual of useful procedures, nor should it be expected to provide mechanical procedures for the discovery of grammars" - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Sampson 1980, pp. 76 notes that "the fullest and most interesting expression of 'discovery procedure' is [the] book Methods in Structural Linguistics (Harris 1951) by Zellig Harris, Chomsky's mentor. - Sampson, Geoffrey (1980), Schools of Linguistics, London: Hutchinson & Co
Chomsky 1957, pp. 49–56 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
See Chomsky 1978, pp. 9–10. Chomsky characterized this approach as the "Galilean Style" of inquiry which had already been applied in modern natural sciences with "great success" since the 17th century. - Chomsky, Noam (1978), "A theory of core grammar", Glot (1): 7–26
Chomsky 1957, p. 68 states:"a wide variety of apparently distinct phenomena [in English language] all fall into place in a very simple and natural way when we adopt the viewpoint of transformational analysis and that, consequently, the grammar of English becomes much more simple and orderly." - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, pp. 85–87 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 91 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 93 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, p. 101 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, pp. 96–97 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Chomsky 1957, pp. 96–97 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Because it would "reveal" insights about sentence structures. See Chomsky 1957, p. 103 - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Harris 1993, Chapter 3 - Harris, Randy Allen (1993), The Linguistics Wars, New York: Oxford University Press, ISBN 9780195098341 https://global.oup.com/academic/product/the-linguistics-wars-9780195098341?cc=gb&lang=en&
Harris 1989 - Harris, Randy Allen (1989), "Argumentation in Chomsky's syntactic structures: An exercise in rhetoric of science", Rhetoric Society Quarterly, 19 (2): 105–130, doi:10.1080/02773948909390840 https://doi.org/10.1080%2F02773948909390840
Newmeyer 1987, p. 24 wrote that “[Chomsky’s] examples of defects of phrase structure grammar were illustrated simultaneously with the demonstration that grammars containing the more powerful transformational rules can handle the same phenomena in an elegant and revealing manner.” - Newmeyer, Frederick J. (1987), Linguistic Theory in America (2 ed.), San Diego, California: Academic Press, ISBN 978-0125171519 https://books.google.com/books?id=-ySLBQAAQBAJ
According to Brown 2010, p. 188, "this apparently curious analysis is rather ingenious" and "the powerful tool of different levels of structure related by transformations was particularly beguiling, since transformations appeared to offer a means of explaining the often amazingly complex relationships between the forms of sentences and their understanding." - Brown, E. Keith (2010), "Generative Grammar", in Kirsten Malmkjaer (ed.), The Routledge Linguistics Encyclopedia (3 ed.), London & New York: Routledge
In his introduction to Syntactic Structures (Chomsky 1957, p. ix), American linguist David Lightfoot wrote that "this ingenious transformation...avoided hopelessly complex phrase structure rules and yielded an elegant account... ” - Chomsky, Noam (1957), Syntactic Structures, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, ISBN 978-3-11-021832-9 https://books.google.com/books?id=a6a_b-CXYAkC
Newmeyer 1987, p. 24 - Newmeyer, Frederick J. (1987), Linguistic Theory in America (2 ed.), San Diego, California: Academic Press, ISBN 978-0125171519 https://books.google.com/books?id=-ySLBQAAQBAJ
Brown 2010, p. 186 - Brown, E. Keith (2010), "Generative Grammar", in Kirsten Malmkjaer (ed.), The Routledge Linguistics Encyclopedia (3 ed.), London & New York: Routledge
Aronoff 2014 - Aronoff, Mark (2014), "Face the facts : Reading Chomsky's 'Remarks on Nominalization' after forty years", in Florence Villoing; Sophie David (eds.), Foisonnements morphologiques: études en hommage à Françoise Kerleroux, Paris: Presses Universitaires de Paris Ouest, pp. 307–324, archived from the original on 2016-06-09, retrieved 2017-10-08 https://web.archive.org/web/20160609063753/https://linguistics.stonybrook.edu/sites/default/files/uploads/u6/publications/Aronoff%20Face%20the%20facts.doc
Oenbring 2009 - Oenbring, Raymond (2009), Scientific rhetoric and disciplinary identity: A critical rhetorical history of generative grammar (Ph.D. thesis), University of Washington
Voegelin 1958 - Voegelin, Charles F (1958), "Review of Noam Chomsky, Syntactic Structures", International Journal of American Linguistics, 24 (3): 229–231, doi:10.1086/464460 https://doi.org/10.1086%2F464460
Joos 1961 - Joos, Martin (1961), "Linguistic prospects in the US", in Christine Mohrmann; et al. (eds.), Trends in European and American Linguistics, Utrecht: Spectrum, pp. 11–20
Postal 1964 - Postal, Paul M. (1964), Constituent Structure: A Study of Contemporary Models of Syntactic Description, Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University
Levin 1965, p. 92 - Levin, Samuel R. (1965), "Langue and Parole in American Linguistics", Foundations of Language, 1: 83–94
Bach 1965, pp. 111–12 - Bach, Emmon (1965), "Structural Linguistics and the Philosophy of Science", Diogenes, 13 (51): 111–128, doi:10.1177/039219216501305107, S2CID 145793812 https://scholarworks.umass.edu/linguist_faculty_pubs/155
Thorne 1965, p. 74 - Thorne, James Peter (1965), "Review of P. Postal, Constituent Structure", Journal of Linguistics, 1: 73–6, doi:10.1017/s0022226700001055, S2CID 144201727 https://doi.org/10.1017%2Fs0022226700001055
Lyons 1966 - Lyons, John (1966), "Review of Aspects of the Theory of Syntax by Noam Chomsky", The Philosophical Quarterly, 16 (65): 393–395, doi:10.2307/2218520, JSTOR 2218520. https://doi.org/10.2307%2F2218520
Robins 1967, p. 226 - Robins, R. H. (1967), A Short History of Linguistics, London: Longman, ISBN 9781317891116 https://books.google.com/books?id=HKYuAgAAQBAJ
Newmeyer 1996, pp. 24–26 - Newmeyer, Frederick J. (1996), Generative Linguistics: A Historical Perspective, Routledge history of linguistic thought series, Routledge, ISBN 978-0-415-11553-7 https://books.google.com/books?id=MB8i7owaaNYC
Norbert Hornstein (27 January 2017). "On Syntactic Structures". Faculty of Language. Retrieved 18 July 2017. http://facultyoflanguage.blogspot.com/2017/01/on-syntactic-structures.html
Chomsky 1959 - Chomsky, Noam (1959), "A Review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior", Language, 35 (1): 26–58, doi:10.2307/411334, JSTOR 411334 http://cogprints.org/1148/1/chomsky.htm
Skinner 1957 - Skinner, Burrhus Frederick (1957), Verbal Behavior, Acton, MA: Copley Publishing Group, ISBN 978-1-58390-021-5
Chomsky 1959 - Chomsky, Noam (1959), "A Review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior", Language, 35 (1): 26–58, doi:10.2307/411334, JSTOR 411334 http://cogprints.org/1148/1/chomsky.htm
According to Steinberg, Hiroshi & Aline 2013, p. 371: "[Chomsky's generative system of rules] was more powerful that anything ... psycholinguists had heretofore had at their disposal. [It] was of special interest to these theorists. Many psychologists were quick to attribute generative systems to the minds of speakers and quick to abandon ... Behaviorism." - Steinberg, Danny D.; Hiroshi, Nagata; Aline, David P. (2013), Psycholinguistics: Language, Mind and World, Routledge, ISBN 9781317900566 https://books.google.com/books?id=EfK1AQAAQBAJ
Searle 1972 - Searle, John R. (1972), "Chomsky's Revolution in Linguistics", The New York Review of Books, 18 (12) http://www.nybooks.com/articles/10142
Quine 1969 - Quine, Willard Van Orman (1969), "Reply to Chomsky", in D. Davidson and J. Hintikka (ed.), Words and Objections, Dordrecht: D. Reidel
Stokhof 2012, p. 548 writes: "That natural languages are indeed not systematic enough to allow formal treatment ... is ... a complaint that has been leveled against natural languages by philosophers for centuries. The work of Chomsky in generative linguistics apparently inspired much more confidence in philosophers and logicians to assert that perhaps natural languages weren't as unsystematic and misleading as their philosophical predecessors had made them out to be ... at the end of 1960s formal semantics began to flourish." - Stokhof, Martin (2012), "The Role of Artificial Languages in the Philosophy of Language", in Gillian Russell and Delia Graff Fara (ed.), The Routledge Companion to Philosophy of Language, Routledge Philosophy Companions, Routledge https://www.routledge.com/Routledge-Companion-to-Philosophy-of-Language/Russell-Fara/p/book/9780415993104
Davidson 1967 writes: "Recent work by Chomsky and others is doing much to bring the complexities of natural languages within the scope of serious semantic theory". - Davidson, Donald (1967), "Truth and Meaning", Synthese, 17: 304–23, doi:10.1007/bf00485035, S2CID 14720789 https://doi.org/10.1007%2Fbf00485035
From the preface of Knuth 2003: "... researchers in linguistics were beginning to formulate rules of grammar that were considerably more mathematical than before. And people began to realize that such methods are highly relevant to the artificial languages that were becoming popular for computer programming, even though natural languages like English remained intractable. I found the mathematical approach to grammar immediately appealing—so much so, in fact, that I must admit to taking a copy of Noam Chomsky's Syntactic Structures along with me on my honeymoon in 1961. During odd moments, while crossing the Atlantic in an ocean liner and while camping in Europe, I read that book rather thoroughly and tried to answer some basic theoretical questions. Here was a marvelous thing: a mathematical theory of language in which I could use a computer programmer's intuition! The mathematical, linguistic, and algorithmic parts of my life had previously been totally separate. During the ensuing years those three aspects became steadily more intertwined; and by the end of the 1960s I found myself a Professor of Computer Science at Stanford University, primarily because of work that I had done with respect to languages for computer programming." - Knuth, Donald (2003), Selected Papers on Computer Languages, CSLI Lecture Notes, Stanford, California: Center for the Study of Language and Information, archived from the original on 2018-08-20, retrieved 2009-09-17 https://web.archive.org/web/20180820080806/https://www-cs-faculty.stanford.edu/~knuth/cl.html
Boden 2006, p. 648 writes:"[Papers like the "Three Models"] had a huge, lasting influence on pure computer science" and that they are cited in "virtually every introduction to compiler design". Hopcroft & Ullman 1979, p. 9 states that "Chomsky's notion of a context-free grammar ... has aided immensely the specification of programming languages." - Boden, Margaret (2006), Mind as Machine: A History of Cognitive Science, Oxford: Oxford University Press, ISBN 978-0-19-924144-6 https://books.google.com/books?id=b4SE3C8PYU0C
Pallier, Devauchelle & Dehaene 2011, pp. 2522–2527 - Pallier, Christophe; Devauchelle, Anne-Dominique; Dehaene, Stanislas (2011), "Cortical representation of the constituent structure of sentences", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108 (6): 2522–2527, doi:10.1073/pnas.1018711108, PMC 3038732, PMID 21224415 https://doi.org/10.1073%2Fpnas.1018711108
"Chomsky Was Right, NYU Researchers Find: We Do Have a "Grammar" in Our Head". New York University. Retrieved 16 November 2016. http://www.nyu.edu/about/news-publications/news/2015/december/chomsky-was-right-nyu-researchers-find-we-do-have-a-grammar-in-our-head.html
Hockett 1965, p. 185 - Hockett, Charles (1965), "Sound Change", Language, 41 (2): 185–204, doi:10.2307/411873, JSTOR 411873. https://doi.org/10.2307%2F411873
The other three are Sir William Jones's address to the Asiatic Society in 1786, Karl Verner's Eine Ausnahme der ersten Lautverschiebung in 1875 and Ferdinand de Saussure's Cours de Linguistique Générale in 1916. /wiki/Sir_William_Jones
Hockett 1966, p. 156 - Hockett, Charles (1966), "Language, mathematics and linguistics", Current Trends in Linguistics, vol. 3. Theoretical Foundations, The Hague: Mouton, pp. 155–304 https://archive.org/details/languagemathemat00hock
Hockett 1968, p. 83 states: "we must not promote our more or less standardized by-and-large characterization of the language to the status of a monolithic ideal, nor infer that because we can achieve a fixed characterization some such monolithic ideal exists, in the lap of God or in the brain of each individual speaker." - Hockett, Charles (1968), The State of the Art, The Hague: Mouton
Hockett 1968, pp. 67–71 - Hockett, Charles (1968), The State of the Art, The Hague: Mouton
Sampson 1980, p. 134. - Sampson, Geoffrey (1980), Schools of Linguistics, London: Hutchinson & Co
Sampson 2001, pp. 5, 10, 13. - Sampson, Geoffrey (2001), Empirical Linguistics, London and New York: Continuum International http://www.grsampson.net/BEmpLj.html
Sampson 2001, p. 152 - Sampson, Geoffrey (2001), Empirical Linguistics, London and New York: Continuum International http://www.grsampson.net/BEmpLj.html
Pullum 2011 - Pullum, Geoffrey K. (2011), "On the Mathematical Foundations of Syntactic Structures" (PDF), Journal of Logic, Language and Information, 20 (3): 277–296, doi:10.1007/s10849-011-9139-8, hdl:20.500.11820/3bfafa09-b7f2-4249-b2d3-714227f2ddb8, S2CID 26842058 http://www.lel.ed.ac.uk/~gpullum/onSyntacticStructures.pdf
Pullum 2011 - Pullum, Geoffrey K. (2011), "On the Mathematical Foundations of Syntactic Structures" (PDF), Journal of Logic, Language and Information, 20 (3): 277–296, doi:10.1007/s10849-011-9139-8, hdl:20.500.11820/3bfafa09-b7f2-4249-b2d3-714227f2ddb8, S2CID 26842058 http://www.lel.ed.ac.uk/~gpullum/onSyntacticStructures.pdf
Pullum 2011 - Pullum, Geoffrey K. (2011), "On the Mathematical Foundations of Syntactic Structures" (PDF), Journal of Logic, Language and Information, 20 (3): 277–296, doi:10.1007/s10849-011-9139-8, hdl:20.500.11820/3bfafa09-b7f2-4249-b2d3-714227f2ddb8, S2CID 26842058 http://www.lel.ed.ac.uk/~gpullum/onSyntacticStructures.pdf
Pullum 2011 - Pullum, Geoffrey K. (2011), "On the Mathematical Foundations of Syntactic Structures" (PDF), Journal of Logic, Language and Information, 20 (3): 277–296, doi:10.1007/s10849-011-9139-8, hdl:20.500.11820/3bfafa09-b7f2-4249-b2d3-714227f2ddb8, S2CID 26842058 http://www.lel.ed.ac.uk/~gpullum/onSyntacticStructures.pdf
Pullum & Gazdar 1982 - Pullum, Geoffrey K.; Gazdar, Gerald (1982), "Natural languages and context-free languages" (PDF), Linguistics and Philosophy, 4 (4): 471–504, doi:10.1007/bf00360802, S2CID 189881482, archived from the original (PDF) on 2019-11-01, retrieved 2016-10-22 https://web.archive.org/web/20191101073516/http://lingo.stanford.edu/sag/papers/pg82.pdf
Versions of such non-transformational phrase structure grammars include Generalized phrase structure grammar (GPSG), Head-driven phrase structure grammar (HPSG) and Lexical functional grammar (LFG). /wiki/Generalized_phrase_structure_grammar
See the list of the 100 most influential works in cognitive science from the 20th century online here: https://web.archive.org/web/20040821111702/http://www.cogsci.umn.edu/OLD/calendar/past_events/millennium/final.html https://web.archive.org/web/20040821111702/http://www.cogsci.umn.edu/OLD/calendar/past_events/millennium/final.html
Seymour-Smith 1998 - Seymour-Smith, Martin (1998), The 100 most influential books ever written : the history of thought from ancient times to today, Secaucus, N.J.: Carol Publ. Group, ISBN 978-0-8065-2000-1, OCLC 38258131 https://search.worldcat.org/oclc/38258131
Grossman, Lev (17 August 2016). "All-TIME 100 Nonfiction Books : Syntactic Structures". Time. Retrieved 14 October 2016. https://entertainment.time.com/2011/08/30/all-time-100-best-nonfiction-books/slide/syntactic-structures-by-noam-chomsky/