Menu
Home Explore People Places Arts History Plants & Animals Science Life & Culture Technology
On this page
Big Five personality traits
Personality model consisting of five broad dimensions of personality

The trait theory identifies the Big Five personality traits—openness to experience, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism—as key factors in understanding human personality. These traits emerged empirically from lexical studies using factor analysis, rather than from a pre-existing theory. While broadly embraced, the model faces critiques, including suggestions to replace it with the HEXACO model, which adds a sixth Honesty-Humility factor. Despite some debate, the Big Five remains central to contemporary personality research, though scholars like Jack Block caution against its uncritical acceptance.

Related Image Collections Add Image
We don't have any YouTube videos related to Big Five personality traits yet.
We don't have any PDF documents related to Big Five personality traits yet.
We don't have any Books related to Big Five personality traits yet.
We don't have any archived web articles related to Big Five personality traits yet.

History

William McDougall, writing in 1932, put forward a conjecture observing that "five distinguishable but separable factors" could be identified when looking at personality. His suggestions, "intellect, character, temperament, disposition and temper", have been seen as "anticipating" the adoption of the Big Five model in subsequent years.6 The model was built on understanding the relationship between personality and academic behaviour.7 It was defined by several independent sets of researchers who analysed words describing people's behaviour.8 These researchers first studied relationships between many words related to personality traits. They made lists of these words shorter by 5–10 times and then used factor analysis to group the remaining traits (with data mostly based upon people's estimations, in self-report questionnaires and peer ratings) to find the basic factors of personality.910111213

The initial model was advanced in 1958 by Ernest Tupes and Raymond Christal, research psychologists at the Lackland Air Force Base in Texas, but failed to reach scholars and scientists until the 1980s. In 1990, J.M. Digman advanced his five-factor model of personality, which Lewis Goldberg put at the highest organised level.14 These five overarching domains have been found to contain most known personality traits and are assumed to represent the basic structure behind them all.15

At least four sets of researchers have worked independently for decades to reflect personality traits in language and have mainly identified the same five factors: Tupes and Christal were first, followed by Goldberg at the Oregon Research Institute,1617181920 Cattell at the University of Illinois,21222324 and finally Costa and McCrae.25262728 These four sets of researchers used somewhat different methods in finding the five traits, making the sets of five factors have varying names and meanings. However, all have been found to be strongly correlated with their corresponding factors.2930313233 Studies indicate that the Big Five traits are not nearly as powerful in predicting and explaining actual behaviour as the more numerous facets or primary traits.3435

Each of the Big Five personality traits contains two separate, but correlated, aspects reflecting a level of personality below the broad domains but above the many facet scales also making up part of the Big Five.36 The aspects are labelled as follows: Volatility and Withdrawal for Neuroticism; Enthusiasm and Assertiveness for Extraversion; Intellect and Openness for Openness to Experience; Industriousness and Orderliness for Conscientiousness; and Compassion and Politeness for Agreeableness.37

Finding the five factors

In 1884, British scientist Sir Francis Galton became the first person known to consider deriving a comprehensive taxonomy of human personality traits by sampling language.38 The idea that this may be possible is known as the lexical hypothesis. In 1936, American psychologists Gordon Allport of Harvard University and Henry Odbert of Dartmouth College implemented Galton's hypothesis. They organised for three anonymous people to categorise adjectives from Webster's New International Dictionary and a list of common slang words. The result was a list of 4504 adjectives they believed were descriptive of observable and relatively permanent traits.39

In 1943, Raymond Cattell of Harvard University took Allport and Odbert's list and reduced this to a list of roughly 160 terms by eliminating words with very similar meanings. To these, he added terms from 22 other psychological categories, and additional "interest" and "abilities" terms. This resulted in a list of 171 traits. From this he used factor analysis to derive 60 "personality clusters or syndromes" and an additional 7 minor clusters.40 Cattell then narrowed this down to 35 terms, and later added a 36th factor in the form of an IQ measure. Through factor analysis from 1945 to 1948, he created 11 or 12 factor solutions.414243

In 1947, Hans Eysenck of University College London published his book Dimensions of Personality. He posited that the two most important personality dimensions were "Extraversion" and "Neuroticism", a term that he coined.44

In July 1949, Donald Fiske of the University of Chicago used 22 terms either adapted from Cattell's 1947 study, and through surveys of male university students and statistics derived five factors: "Social Adaptability", "Emotional Control", "Conformity", "Inquiring Intellect", and "Confident Self-expression".45 In the same year, Cattell, with Maurice Tatsuoka and Herbert Eber, found 4 additional factors, which they believed consisted of information that could only be provided through self-rating. With this understanding, they created the sixteen factor 16PF Questionnaire.4647484950

In 1953, John W French of Educational Testing Service published an extensive meta-analysis of personality trait factor studies.51

In 1957, Ernest Tupes of the United States Air Force undertook a personality trait study of US Air Force officers. Each was rated by their peers using Cattell's 35 terms (or in some cases, the 30 most reliable terms).5253 In 1958, Tupes and Raymond Christal began a US Air Force study by taking 37 personality factors and other data found in Cattell's 1947 paper, Fiske's 1949 paper, and Tupes' 1957 paper.54 Through statistical analysis, they derived five factors they labeled "Surgency", "Agreeableness", "Dependability", "Emotional Stability", and "Culture".5556 In addition to the influence of Cattell and Fiske's work, they strongly noted the influence of French's 1953 study.57 Tupes and Christal further tested and explained their 1958 work in a 1961 paper.5859

Warren Norman60 of the University of Michigan replicated Tupes and Christal's work in 1963. He relabeled "Surgency" as "Extroversion or Surgency", and "Dependability" as "Conscientiousness". He also found four subordinate scales for each factor.61 Norman's paper was much more read than Tupes and Christal's papers had been. Norman's later Oregon Research Institute colleague Lewis Goldberg continued this work.62

In the 4th edition of the 16PF Questionnaire released in 1968, 5 "global factors" derived from the 16 factors were identified: "Extraversion", "Independence", "Anxiety", "Self-control" and "Tough-mindedness".63 16PF advocates have since called these "the original Big 5".64

Hiatus in research

During the 1970s, the changing zeitgeist made publication of personality research difficult. In his 1968 book Personality and Assessment, Walter Mischel asserted that personality instruments could not predict behavior with a correlation of more than 0.3. Social psychologists like Mischel argued that attitudes and behavior were not stable, but varied with the situation. Predicting behavior from personality instruments was claimed to be impossible.[]

Renewed attention

In 1978, Paul Costa and Robert McCrae of the National Institutes of Health published a book chapter describing their Neuroticism-Extroversion-Openness (NEO) model. The model was based on the three factors in its name.65 They used Eysenck's concept of "Extroversion" rather than Carl Jung's.66 Each factor had six facets. The authors expanded their explanation of the model in subsequent papers.

Also in 1978, British psychologist Peter Saville of Brunel University applied statistical analysis to 16PF results, and determined that the model could be reduced to five factors, "Anxiety", "Extraversion", "Warmth", "Imagination" and "Conscientiousness".67

At a 1980 symposium in Honolulu, Lewis Goldberg, Naomi Takemoto-Chock, Andrew Comrey, and John M. Digman, reviewed the available personality instruments of the day.68 In 1981, Digman and Takemoto-Chock of the University of Hawaii reanalysed data from Cattell, Tupes, Norman, Fiske and Digman. They re-affirmed the validity of the five factors, naming them "Friendly Compliance vs. Hostile Non-compliance", "Extraversion vs. Introversion", "Ego Strength vs. Emotional Disorganization", "Will to Achieve" and "Intellect". They also found weak evidence for the existence of a sixth factor, "Culture".69

Peter Saville and his team included the five-factor "Pentagon" model as part of the Occupational Personality Questionnaires (OPQ) in 1984. This was the first commercially available Big Five test.70 Its factors are "Extroversion", "Vigorous", "Methodical", "Emotional Stability", and "Abstract".71

This was closely followed by another commercial test, the NEO PI three-factor personality inventory, published by Costa and McCrae in 1985. It used the three NEO factors. The methodology employed in constructing the NEO instruments has since been subject to critical scrutiny.72: 431–33 

Emerging methodologies increasingly confirmed personality theories during the 1980s. Though generally failing to predict single instances of behavior, researchers found that they could predict patterns of behavior by aggregating large numbers of observations.73 As a result, correlations between personality and behavior increased substantially, and it became clear that "personality" did in fact exist.74

In 1992, the NEO PI evolved into the NEO PI-R, adding the factors "Agreeableness" and "Conscientiousness",75 and becoming a Big Five instrument. This set the names for the factors that are now most commonly used. The NEO maintainers call their model the "Five Factor Model" (FFM). Each NEO personality dimension has six subordinate facets.

Subsequent developments

Wim Hofstee at the University of Groningen used a lexical hypothesis approach with the Dutch language to develop what became the International Personality Item Pool in the 1990s. Further development in Germany and the United States saw the pool based on three languages. Its questions and results have been mapped to various Big Five personality typing models.7677

Kibeom Lee and Michael Ashton released a book describing their HEXACO model in 2004.78 It adds a sixth factor, "Honesty-Humility" to the five (which it calls "Emotionality", "Extraversion", "Agreeableness", "Conscientiousness", and "Openness to Experience"). Each of these factors has four facets.

In 2007, Colin DeYoung, Lena C. Quilty and Jordan Peterson concluded that the 10 aspects of the Big Five may have distinct biological substrates.79 This was derived through factor analyses of two data samples with the International Personality Item Pool, followed by cross-correlation with scores derived from 10 genetic factors identified as underlying the shared variance among the Revised NEO Personality Inventory facets.80

By 2009, personality and social psychologists generally agreed that both personal and situational variables are needed to account for human behavior.81

A FFM-associated test was used by Cambridge Analytica, and was part of the "psychographic profiling"82 controversy during the 2016 US presidential election.8384

Descriptions of the particular personality traits

When factor analysis is applied to personality survey data, semantic associations between aspects of personality and specific terms are often applied to the same person. For example, someone described as conscientious is more likely to be described as "always prepared" rather than "messy". These associations suggest five broad dimensions used in common language to describe the human personality, temperament, and psyche.8586

Beneath each proposed global factor, there are a number of correlated and more specific primary factors. For example, extraversion is typically associated with qualities such as gregariousness, assertiveness, excitement-seeking, warmth, activity, and positive emotions.87 These traits are not black and white; each one is treated as a spectrum.88

Openness to experience

Openness to experience is a general appreciation for art, emotion, adventure, unusual ideas, imagination, curiosity, and variety of experience. People who are open to experience are intellectually curious, open to emotion, sensitive to beauty, and willing to try new things. They tend to be, when compared to closed people, more creative and more aware of their feelings. They are also more likely to hold unconventional beliefs. Open people can be perceived as unpredictable or lacking focus, and more likely to engage in risky behaviour or drug-taking.89 Moreover, individuals with high openness are said to pursue self-actualisation specifically by seeking out intense, euphoric experiences. Conversely, those with low openness want to be fulfilled by persevering and are characterised as pragmatic and data-driven – sometimes even perceived to be dogmatic and closed-minded. Some disagreement remains about how to interpret and contextualise the openness factor as there is a lack of biological support for this particular trait. Openness has not shown a significant association with any brain regions as opposed to the other four traits which did when using brain imaging to detect changes in volume associated with each trait.90

Sample items

  • I have a rich vocabulary.
  • I have a vivid imagination.
  • I have excellent ideas.
  • I am quick to understand things.
  • I use difficult words.
  • I spend time reflecting on things.
  • I am full of ideas.
  • I cherish imaginations
  • I have difficulty understanding abstract ideas. (Reversed)
  • I am not interested in abstract ideas. (Reversed)
  • I do not have a good imagination. (Reversed)91

Conscientiousness

Conscientiousness is a tendency to be self-disciplined, act dutifully, and strive for achievement against measures or outside expectations. It is related to people's level of impulse control, regulation, and direction. High conscientiousness is often perceived as being stubborn and focused. Low conscientiousness is associated with flexibility and spontaneity, but can also appear as sloppiness and lack of reliability.92 High conscientiousness indicates a preference for planned rather than spontaneous behaviour.93

Sample items

  • I am always prepared.
  • I pay attention to details.
  • I get chores done right away.
  • I follow a schedule.
  • I am exacting in my work.
  • I do not like order. (Reversed)
  • I leave my belongings around. (Reversed)
  • I make a mess of things. (Reversed)
  • I often forget to put things back in their proper place. (Reversed)
  • I shirk my duties. (Reversed)94

Extraversion

Extraversion is characterised by breadth of activities (as opposed to depth), surgency from external activities/situations, and energy creation from external means.95 The trait is marked by pronounced engagement with the external world. Extraverts enjoy interacting with people, and are often perceived as energetic. They tend to be enthusiastic and action-oriented. They possess high group visibility, like to talk, and assert themselves. Extraverts may appear more dominant in social settings, as opposed to introverts in that setting.96

Introverts have lower social engagement and energy levels than extraverts. They tend to seem quiet, low-key, deliberate, and less involved in the social world. Their lack of social involvement should not be interpreted as shyness or depression, but as greater independence of their social world than extraverts. Introverts need less stimulation and more time alone than extraverts. This does not mean that they are unfriendly or antisocial; rather, they are aloof and reserved in social situations.97

Generally, people are a combination of extraversion and introversion, with personality psychologist Hans Eysenck suggesting a model by which differences in their brains produce these traits.98: 106 

Sample items

  • I am the life of the party.
  • I feel comfortable around people.
  • I start conversations.
  • I talk to a lot of different people at parties.
  • I do not mind being the center of attention.
  • I do not talk a lot. (Reversed)
  • I keep in the background. (Reversed)
  • I have little to say. (Reversed)
  • I do not like to draw attention to myself. (Reversed)
  • I am quiet around strangers. (Reversed)99

Agreeableness

Agreeableness is the general concern for social harmony. Agreeable individuals value getting along with others. They are generally considerate, kind, generous, trusting and trustworthy, helpful, and willing to compromise their interests with others.100 Agreeable people also have an optimistic view of human nature. Being agreeable helps us cope with stress.101

Disagreeable individuals place self-interest above getting along with others. They are generally unconcerned with others' well-being and are less likely to extend themselves for other people. Sometimes their skepticism about others' motives causes them to be suspicious, unfriendly, and uncooperative.102 Disagreeable people are often competitive or challenging, which can be seen as argumentative or untrustworthy.103

Because agreeableness is a social trait, research has shown that one's agreeableness positively correlates with the quality of relationships with one's team members. Agreeableness also positively predicts transformational leadership skills. In a study conducted among 169 participants in leadership positions in a variety of professions, individuals were asked to take a personality test and be directly evaluated by supervised subordinates. Very agreeable leaders were more likely to be considered transformational rather than transactional. Although the relationship was not strong (r=0.32, β=0.28, p<0.01), it was the strongest of the Big Five traits. However, the same study could not predict leadership effectiveness as evaluated by the leader's direct supervisor.104

Conversely, agreeableness has been found to be negatively related to transactional leadership in the military. A study of Asian military units showed that agreeable people are more likely to be poor transactional leaders.105 Therefore, with further research, organisations may be able to determine an individual's potential for performance based on their personality traits. For instance,106 in their journal article "Which Personality Attributes Are Most Important in the Workplace?" Paul Sackett and Philip Walmsley claim that conscientiousness and agreeableness are "important to success across many different jobs."

Sample items

  • I am interested in people.
  • I sympathise with others' feelings.
  • I have a soft heart.
  • I take time out for others.
  • I feel others' emotions.
  • I make people feel at ease.
  • I am not really interested in others. (Reversed)
  • I insult people. (Reversed)
  • I am not interested in other people's problems. (Reversed)
  • I feel little concern for others. (Reversed)107

Neuroticism

Neuroticism is the tendency to have strong negative emotions, such as anger, anxiety, or depression.108 It is sometimes called emotional instability, or is reversed and referred to as emotional stability. According to Hans Eysenck's (1967) theory of personality, neuroticism is associated with low tolerance for stress or a strong dislike of change.109 Neuroticism is a classic temperament trait that has been studied in temperament research for decades, even before it was adapted by the Five Factor Model.110 Neurotic people are emotionally reactive and vulnerable to stress. They are more likely to interpret ordinary situations as threatening. They can perceive minor frustrations as hopelessly difficult. Their negative emotional reactions tend to stay for unusually long periods of time, which means they are often in a bad mood. For instance, neuroticism is connected to pessimism toward work, to certainty that work hinders personal relationships, and to higher levels of anxiety from the pressures at work.111 Furthermore, neurotic people may display more skin-conductance reactivity than calm and composed people.112113 These problems in emotional regulation can make a neurotic person think less clearly, make worse decisions, and cope less effectively with stress. Being disappointed with one's life achievements can make one more neurotic and increase one's chances of falling into clinical depression. Moreover, neurotic individuals tend to experience more negative life events,114115 but neuroticism also changes in response to positive and negative life experiences.116117 Also, neurotic people tend to have worse psychological well-being.118

At the other end of the scale, less neurotic individuals are less easily upset and are less emotionally reactive. They tend to be calm, emotionally stable, and free from persistent negative feelings. Freedom from negative feelings does not mean that low scorers experience a lot of positive feelings; that is related to extraversion instead.119

Neuroticism is similar but not identical to being neurotic in the Freudian sense (i.e., neurosis). Some psychologists[] prefer to call neuroticism by the term emotional instability to differentiate it from the term neurotic in a career test.

Sample items

  • I get stressed out easily.
  • I worry about things.
  • I am easily disturbed.
  • I get upset easily.
  • I change my mood a lot.
  • I have frequent mood swings.
  • I get irritated easily.
  • I often feel blue.
  • I am relaxed most of the time. (Reversed)
  • I seldom feel blue. (Reversed)120

Biological and developmental factors

The factors that influence a personality are called the determinants of personality. These factors determine the traits which a person develops in the course of development from a child.

Temperament and personality

There are debates between temperament researchers and personality researchers as to whether or not biologically based differences define a concept of temperament or a part of personality. The presence of such differences in pre-cultural individuals (such as animals or young infants) suggests that they belong to temperament since personality is a socio-cultural concept. For this reason developmental psychologists generally interpret individual differences in children as an expression of temperament rather than personality.121 Some researchers argue that temperaments and personality traits are age-specific demonstrations of virtually the same internal qualities.122123 Some believe that early childhood temperaments may become adolescent and adult personality traits as individuals' basic genetic characteristics interact with their changing environments to various degrees.124125126

Researchers of adult temperament point out that, similarly to sex, age, and mental illness, temperament is based on biochemical systems whereas personality is a product of socialisation of an individual possessing these four types of features. Temperament interacts with socio-cultural factors, but, similar to sex and age, still cannot be controlled or easily changed by these factors.127128129130 Therefore, it is suggested that temperament (neurochemically based individual differences) should be kept as an independent concept for further studies and not be confused with personality (culturally-based individual differences, reflected in the origin of the word "persona" (Lat) as a "social mask").131132

Moreover, temperament refers to dynamic features of behaviour (energetic, tempo, sensitivity, and emotionality-related), whereas personality is to be considered a psycho-social construct comprising the content characteristics of human behaviour (such as values, attitudes, habits, preferences, personal history, self-image).133134135 Temperament researchers point out that the lack of attention to surviving temperament research by the creators of the Big Five model led to an overlap between its dimensions and dimensions described in multiple temperament models much earlier. For example, neuroticism reflects the traditional temperament dimension of emotionality studied by Jerome Kagan's group since the '60s. Extraversion was also first introduced as a temperament type by Jung from the '20s.136137

Heritability

A 1996 behavioural genetics study of twins suggested that heritability (the degree of variation in a trait within a population that is due to genetic variation in that population) and environmental factors both influence all five factors to the same degree.138 Among four twin studies examined in 2003, the mean percentage for heritability was calculated for each personality and it was concluded that heritability influenced the five factors broadly. The self-report measures were as follows: openness to experience was estimated to have a 57% genetic influence, extraversion 54%, conscientiousness 49%, neuroticism 48%, and agreeableness 42%.139

Non-humans

The Big Five personality traits have been assessed in some non-human species but methodology is debatable. In one series of studies, human ratings of chimpanzees using the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire, revealed factors of extraversion, conscientiousness and agreeableness– as well as an additional factor of dominance–across hundreds of chimpanzees in zoological parks, a large naturalistic sanctuary, and a research laboratory. Neuroticism and openness factors were found in an original zoo sample, but were not replicated in a new zoo sample or in other settings (perhaps reflecting the design of the CPQ).140 A study review found that markers for the three dimensions extraversion, neuroticism, and agreeableness were found most consistently across different species, followed by openness; only chimpanzees showed markers for conscientious behavior.141

A study completed in 2020 concluded that dolphins have some similar personality traits to humans. Both are large brained intelligent animals but have evolved separately for millions of years.142

Development during childhood and adolescence

Research on the Big Five, and personality in general, has focused primarily on individual differences in adulthood, rather than in childhood and adolescence, and often include temperament traits.143144145 Recently, there has been growing recognition of the need to study child and adolescent personality trait development in order to understand how traits develop and change throughout the lifespan.146

Recent studies have begun to explore the developmental origins and trajectories of the Big Five among children and adolescents, especially those that relate to temperament.147148149 Many researchers have sought to distinguish between personality and temperament.150 Temperament often refers to early behavioral and affective characteristics that are thought to be driven primarily by genes.151 Models of temperament often include four trait dimensions: surgency/sociability, negative emotionality, persistence/effortful control, and activity level.152 Some of these differences in temperament are evident at, if not before, birth.153154 For example, both parents and researchers recognize that some newborn infants are peaceful and easily soothed while others are comparatively fussy and hard to calm.155 Unlike temperament, however, many researchers view the development of personality as gradually occurring throughout childhood.156 Contrary to some researchers who question whether children have stable personality traits, Big Five or otherwise,157 most researchers contend that there are significant psychological differences between children that are associated with relatively stable, distinct, and salient behavior patterns.158159160

The structure, manifestations, and development of the Big Five in childhood and adolescence have been studied using a variety of methods, including parent- and teacher-ratings,161162163 preadolescent and adolescent self- and peer-ratings,164165166 and observations of parent-child interactions.167 Results from these studies support the relative stability of personality traits across the human lifespan, at least from preschool age through adulthood.168169170171 More specifically, research suggests that four of the Big Five – namely Extraversion, Neuroticism, Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness – reliably describe personality differences in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood.172173174175 However, some evidence suggests that Openness may not be a fundamental, stable part of childhood personality. Although some researchers have found that Openness in children and adolescents relates to attributes such as creativity, curiosity, imagination, and intellect,176 many researchers have failed to find distinct individual differences in Openness in childhood and early adolescence.177178 Potentially, Openness may (a) manifest in unique, currently unknown ways in childhood or (b) may only manifest as children develop socially and cognitively.179180 Other studies have found evidence for all of the Big Five traits in childhood and adolescence as well as two other child-specific traits: Irritability and Activity.181 Despite these specific differences, the majority of findings suggest that personality traits – particularly Extraversion, Neuroticism, Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness – are evident in childhood and adolescence and are associated with distinct social-emotional patterns of behavior that are largely consistent with adult manifestations of those same personality traits.182183184185 Some researchers have proposed the youth personality trait is best described by six trait dimensions: neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and activity.186 Despite some preliminary evidence for this "Little Six" model,187188 research in this area has been delayed by a lack of available measures.

Previous research has found evidence that most adults become more agreeable and conscientious and less neurotic as they age.189 This has been referred to as the maturation effect.190 Many researchers have sought to investigate how trends in adult personality development compare to trends in youth personality development.191 Two main population-level indices have been important in this area of research: rank-order consistency and mean-level consistency. Rank-order consistency indicates the relative placement of individuals within a group.192 Mean-level consistency indicates whether groups increase or decrease on certain traits throughout the lifetime.193

Findings from these studies indicate that, consistent with adult personality trends, youth personality becomes increasingly more stable in terms of rank-order throughout childhood.194 Unlike adult personality research, which indicates that people become agreeable, conscientious, and emotionally stable with age,195 some findings in youth personality research have indicated that mean levels of agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness to experience decline from late childhood to late adolescence.196 The disruption hypothesis, which proposes that biological, social, and psychological changes experienced during youth result in temporary dips in maturity, has been proposed to explain these findings.197198

Extraversion/positive emotionality

In Big Five studies, extraversion has been associated with surgency.199 Children with high Extraversion are energetic, talkative, social, and dominant with children and adults, whereas children with low extraversion tend to be quiet, calm, inhibited, and submissive to other children and adults.200 Individual differences in extraversion first manifest in infancy as varying levels of positive emotionality.201 These differences in turn predict social and physical activity during later childhood and may represent, or be associated with, the behavioral activation system.202203 In children, Extraversion/Positive Emotionality includes four sub-traits: three of these (activity, sociability, and shyness) are similar to the previously described traits of temperament;204205 the other is dominance.

  • Activity: Similarly to findings in temperament research, children with high activity tend to have high energy levels and more intense and frequent motor activity compared to their peers.206207208 Salient differences in activity reliably manifest in infancy, persist through adolescence, and fade as motor activity decreases in adulthood209 or potentially develops into talkativeness.210211
  • Dominance: Children with high dominance tend to influence the behavior of others, particularly their peers, to obtain desirable rewards or outcomes.212213214 Such children are generally skilled at organizing activities and games215 and deceiving others by controlling their nonverbal behavior.216
  • Shyness: Children with high shyness are generally socially withdrawn, nervous, and inhibited around strangers.217 In time, such children may become fearful even around "known others", especially if their peers reject them.218219 Similar pattern was described in temperament longitudinal studies of shyness220
  • Sociability: Children with high sociability generally prefer to be with others rather than alone.221222 During middle childhood, the distinction between low sociability and high shyness becomes more pronounced, particularly as children gain greater control over how and where they spend their time.223224225

Development throughout adulthood

Many studies of longitudinal data, which correlate people's test scores over time, and cross-sectional data, which compare personality levels across different age groups, show a high degree of stability in personality traits during adulthood, especially Neuroticism that is often regarded as a temperament trait226 similarly to longitudinal research in temperament for the same traits.227 It is shown that the personality stabilizes for working-age individuals within about four years after starting working. There is also little evidence that adverse life events can have any significant impact on the personality of individuals.228 More recent research and meta-analyses of previous studies, however, indicate that change occurs in all five traits at various points in the lifespan. The new research shows evidence for a maturation effect. On average, levels of agreeableness and conscientiousness typically increase with time, whereas extraversion, neuroticism, and openness tend to decrease.229 Research has also demonstrated that changes in Big Five personality traits depend on the individual's current stage of development. For example, levels of agreeableness and conscientiousness demonstrate a negative trend during childhood and early adolescence before trending upwards during late adolescence and into adulthood.230 In addition to these group effects, there are individual differences: different people demonstrate unique patterns of change at all stages of life.231

In addition, some research (Fleeson, 2001) suggests that the Big Five should not be conceived of as dichotomies (such as extraversion vs. introversion) but as continua. Each individual has the capacity to move along each dimension as circumstances (social or temporal) change. Someone is therefore not simply on one end of each trait dichotomy but is a blend of both, exhibiting some characteristics more often than others:232

Research regarding personality with growing age has suggested that as individuals enter their elder years (79–86), those with lower IQ see a raise in extraversion, but a decline in conscientiousness and physical well-being.233

Group differences

Gender differences

Some cross-cultural research has shown some patterns of gender differences on responses to the NEO-PI-R and the Big Five Inventory.234235 For example, women consistently report higher Neuroticism, Agreeableness, warmth (an extraversion facet) and openness to feelings, and men often report higher assertiveness (a facet of extraversion) and openness to ideas as assessed by the NEO-PI-R.236

A study of gender differences in 55 nations using the Big Five Inventory found that women tended to be somewhat higher than men in neuroticism, extraversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. The difference in neuroticism was the most prominent and consistent, with significant differences found in 49 of the 55 nations surveyed.237

Gender differences in personality traits are largest in prosperous, healthy, and more gender-egalitarian nations. The explanation for this, as stated by the researchers of a 2001 paper, is that actions by women in individualistic, egalitarian countries are more likely to be attributed to their personality, rather than being attributed to ascribed gender roles within collectivist, traditional countries.238

Measured differences in the magnitude of sex differences between more or less developed world regions were caused by the changes in the measured personalities of men, not women, in these respective regions. That is, men in highly developed world regions were less neurotic, less extraverted, less conscientious and less agreeable compared to men in less developed world regions. Women, on the other hand tended not to differ in personality traits across regions.239

Birth-order differences

Main article: Birth order

Frank Sulloway argues that firstborns are more conscientious, more socially dominant, less agreeable, and less open to new ideas compared to siblings that were born later. Large-scale studies using random samples and self-report personality tests, however, have found milder effects than Sulloway claimed, or no significant effects of birth order on personality.240241 A study using the Project Talent data, which is a large-scale representative survey of American high school students, with 272,003 eligible participants, found statistically significant but very small effects (the average absolute correlation between birth order and personality was .02) of birth order on personality, such that firstborns were slightly more conscientious, dominant, and agreeable, while also being less neurotic and less sociable.242 Parental socioeconomic status and participant gender had much larger correlations with personality.

In 2002, the Journal of Psychology posted a Big Five Personality Trait Difference; where researchers explored the relationship between the five-factor model and the Universal-Diverse Orientation (UDO) in counselor trainees. (Thompson, R., Brossart, D., and Mivielle, A., 2002). UDO is known as one social attitude that produces a strong awareness and/or acceptance towards the similarities and differences among individuals. (Miville, M., Romas, J., Johnson, J., and Lon, R. 2002) The study found that the counselor trainees that are more open to the idea of creative expression (a facet of Openness to Experience, Openness to Aesthetics) among individuals are more likely to work with a diverse group of clients, and feel comfortable in their role.243

Cultural differences

Main article: Big Five personality traits and culture

Individual differences in personality traits are widely understood to be conditioned by cultural context.244: 189 

Research into the Big Five has been pursued in a variety of languages and cultures, such as German,245 Chinese,246 and South Asian.247248 For example, Thompson has claimed to find the Big Five structure across several cultures using an international English language scale.249 Cheung, van de Vijver, and Leong (2011) suggest, however, that the Openness factor is particularly unsupported in Asian countries and that a different fifth factor is identified.250

Sopagna Eap et al. (2008) found that European-American men scored higher than Asian-American men on extroversion, conscientiousness, and openness, while Asian-American men scored higher than European-American men on neuroticism.251 Benet-Martínez and Karakitapoglu-Aygün (2003) arrived at similar results.252

Recent work has found relationships between Geert Hofstede's cultural factors, Individualism, Power Distance, Masculinity, and Uncertainty Avoidance, with the average Big Five scores in a country.253 For instance, the degree to which a country values individualism correlates with its average extraversion, whereas people living in cultures which are accepting of large inequalities in their power structures tend to score somewhat higher on conscientiousness.254255

A 2017 study has found that countries' average personality trait levels are correlated with their political systems. Countries with higher average trait Openness tended to have more democratic institutions, an association that held even after factoring out other relevant influences such as economic development.256

Attempts to replicate the Big Five have succeeded in some countries but not in others. Some research suggests, for instance, that Hungarians do not have a single agreeableness factor.257 Other researchers have found evidence for agreeableness but not for other factors.258

Health

Personality and dementia

Some diseases cause changes in personality. For example, although gradual memory impairment is the hallmark feature of Alzheimer's disease, a systematic review of personality changes in Alzheimer's disease by Robins Wahlin and Byrne, published in 2011, found systematic and consistent trait changes mapped to the Big Five. The largest change observed was a decrease in conscientiousness. The next most significant changes were an increase in Neuroticism and decrease in Extraversion, but Openness and Agreeableness were also decreased. These changes in personality could assist with early diagnosis.259

A study published in 2023 found that the Big Five personality traits may also influence the quality of life experienced by people with Alzheimer's disease and other dementias, post diagnosis. In this study people with dementia with lower levels of Neuroticism self-reported higher quality of life than those with higher levels of Neuroticism while those with higher levels of the other four traits self-reported higher quality of life than those with lower levels of these traits. This suggests that as well as assisting with early diagnosis, the Big Five personality traits could help identify people with dementia potentially more vulnerable to adverse outcomes and inform personalized care planning and interventions.260

Personality disorders

Main article: Personality disorders

As of 2002, there were over fifty published studies relating the FFM to personality disorders.261 Since that time, quite a number of additional studies have expanded on this research base and provided further empirical support for understanding the DSM personality disorders in terms of the FFM domains.262

In her review of the personality disorder literature published in 2007, Lee Anna Clark asserted that "the five-factor model of personality is widely accepted as representing the higher-order structure of both normal and abnormal personality traits".263 However, other researchers disagree that this model is widely accepted (see the section Critique below) and suggest that it simply replicates early temperament research.264265 Noticeably, FFM publications never compare their findings to temperament models even though temperament and mental disorders (especially personality disorders) are thought to be based on the same neurotransmitter imbalances, just to varying degrees.266267268269

The five-factor model was claimed to significantly predict all ten personality disorder symptoms and outperform the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) in the prediction of borderline, avoidant, and dependent personality disorder symptoms.270 However, most predictions related to an increase in Neuroticism and a decrease in Agreeableness, and therefore did not differentiate between the disorders very well.271

Common mental disorders

Converging evidence from several nationally representative studies has established three classes of mental disorders which are especially common in the general population: Depressive disorders (e.g., major depressive disorder (MDD), dysthymic disorder),272 anxiety disorders (e.g., generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), panic disorder, agoraphobia, specific phobia, and social phobia),273 and substance use disorders (SUDs).274275 The Five Factor personality profiles of users of different drugs may be different.276 For example, the typical profile for heroin users is N ⇑ , O ⇑ , A ⇓ , C ⇓ {\displaystyle {\rm {N}}\Uparrow ,{\rm {O}}\Uparrow ,{\rm {A}}\Downarrow ,{\rm {C}}\Downarrow } , whereas for ecstasy users the high level of N is not expected but E is higher: E ⇑ , O ⇑ , A ⇓ , C ⇓ {\displaystyle {\rm {E}}\Uparrow ,{\rm {O}}\Uparrow ,{\rm {A}}\Downarrow ,{\rm {C}}\Downarrow } .277

These common mental disorders (CMDs) have been empirically linked to the Big Five personality traits, neuroticism in particular. Numerous studies have found that having high scores of neuroticism significantly increases one's risk for developing a common mental disorder.278279 A large-scale meta-analysis (n > 75,000) examining the relationship between all of the Big Five personality traits and common mental disorders found that low conscientiousness yielded consistently strong effects for each common mental disorder examined (i.e., MDD, dysthymic disorder, GAD, PTSD, panic disorder, agoraphobia, social phobia, specific phobia, and SUD).280 This finding parallels research on physical health, which has established that conscientiousness is the strongest personality predictor of reduced mortality, and is highly negatively correlated with making poor health choices.281282 In regards to the other personality domains, the meta-analysis found that all common mental disorders examined were defined by high neuroticism, most exhibited low extraversion, only SUD was linked to agreeableness (negatively), and no disorders were associated with Openness.283 A meta-analysis of 59 longitudinal studies showed that high neuroticism predicted the development of anxiety, depression, substance abuse, psychosis, schizophrenia, and non-specific mental distress, also after adjustment for baseline symptoms and psychiatric history.284

The personality-psychopathology models

Five major models have been posed to explain the nature of the relationship between personality and mental illness. There is currently no single "best model", as each of them has received at least some empirical support. These models are not mutually exclusive – more than one may be operating for a particular individual and various mental disorders may be explained by different models.285286

  • The Vulnerability/Risk Model: According to this model, personality contributes to the onset or etiology of various common mental disorders. In other words, pre-existing personality traits either cause the development of CMDs directly or enhance the impact of causal risk factors.287288289290 There is strong support for neuroticism being a robust vulnerability factor.291
  • The Pathoplasty Model: This model proposes that premorbid personality traits impact the expression, course, severity, and/or treatment response of a mental disorder.292293294 An example of this relationship would be a heightened likelihood of committing suicide in a depressed individual who also has low levels of constraint.295
  • The Common Cause Model: According to the common cause model, personality traits are predictive of CMDs because personality and psychopathology have shared genetic and environmental determinants which result in non-causal associations between the two constructs.296297
  • The Spectrum Model: This model proposes that associations between personality and psychopathology are found because these two constructs both occupy a single domain or spectrum and psychopathology is simply a display of the extremes of normal personality function.298299300301 Support for this model is provided by an issue of criterion overlap. For instance, two of the primary facet scales of neuroticism in the NEO-PI-R are "depression" and "anxiety". Thus the fact that diagnostic criteria for depression, anxiety, and neuroticism assess the same content increases the correlations between these domains.302
  • The Scar Model: According to the scar model, episodes of a mental disorder 'scar' an individual's personality, changing it in significant ways from premorbid functioning.303304305306 An example of a scar effect would be a decrease in openness to experience following an episode of PTSD.307

Physical health

To examine how the Big Five personality traits are related to subjective health outcomes (positive and negative mood, physical symptoms, and general health concern) and objective health conditions (chronic illness, serious illness, and physical injuries), Jasna Hudek-Knezevic and Igor Kardum conducted a study from a sample of 822 healthy volunteers (438 women and 384 men).308 Out of the Big Five personality traits, they found neuroticism most related to worse subjective health outcomes and optimistic control to better subjective health outcomes. When relating to objective health conditions, connections drawn were presented weak, except that neuroticism significantly predicted chronic illness, whereas optimistic control was more closely related to physical injuries caused by accident.309

Being highly conscientious may add as much as five years to one's life.[]310 The Big Five personality traits also predict positive health outcomes.311 In an elderly Japanese sample, conscientiousness, extraversion, and openness were related to lower risk of mortality.312

Higher conscientiousness is associated with lower obesity risk. In already obese individuals, higher conscientiousness is associated with a higher likelihood of becoming non-obese over a five-year period.313

Effect of personality traits through life

Education

Academic achievement

Personality plays an important role in academic achievement. A study of 308 undergraduates who completed the Five Factor Inventory Processes and reported their GPA suggested that conscientiousness and agreeableness have a positive relationship with all types of learning styles (synthesis-analysis, methodical study, fact retention, and elaborative processing), whereas neuroticism shows an inverse relationship. Moreover, extraversion and openness were proportional to elaborative processing. The Big Five personality traits accounted for 14% of the variance in GPA, suggesting that personality traits make some contributions to academic performance. Furthermore, reflective learning styles (synthesis-analysis and elaborative processing) were able to mediate the relationship between openness and GPA. These results indicate that intellectual curiosity significantly enhances academic performance if students combine their scholarly interest with thoughtful information processing.314

A recent study of Israeli high-school students found that those in the gifted program systematically scored higher on openness and lower on neuroticism than those not in the gifted program. While not a measure of the Big Five, gifted students also reported less state anxiety than students not in the gifted program.315 Specific Big Five personality traits predict learning styles in addition to academic success.

Studies conducted on college students have concluded that hope, which is linked to agreeableness,317 conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness,318 has a positive effect on psychological well-being. Individuals high in neurotic tendencies are less likely to display hopeful tendencies and are negatively associated with well-being.319 Personality can sometimes be flexible and measuring the big five personality for individuals as they enter certain stages of life may predict their educational identity. Recent studies have suggested the likelihood of an individual's personality affecting their educational identity.320

Learning styles

Learning styles have been described as "enduring ways of thinking and processing information".321

In 2008, the Association for Psychological Science (APS) commissioned a report that concludes that no significant evidence exists that learning-style assessments should be included in the education system.322 Thus it is premature, at best, to conclude that the evidence links the Big Five to "learning styles", or "learning styles" to learning itself.

However, the APS report also suggested that all existing learning styles have not been exhausted and that there could exist learning styles worthy of being included in educational practices. There are studies that conclude that personality and thinking styles may be intertwined in ways that link thinking styles to the Big Five personality traits.323 There is no general consensus on the number or specifications of particular learning styles, but there have been many different proposals.

As one example, Schmeck, Ribich, and Ramanaiah (1997) defined four types of learning styles:324

  • synthesis analysis
  • methodical study
  • fact retention
  • elaborative processing

When all four facets are implicated within the classroom, they will each likely improve academic achievement.325 By identifying learning strategies in individuals, learning and academic achievement can be improved, and a deeper understanding of information processing can be gained.326 This model asserts that students develop either agentic/shallow processing or reflective/deep processing. Deep processors are more often found to be more conscientious, intellectually open, and extraverted than shallow processors. Deep processing is associated with appropriate study methods (methodical study) and a stronger ability to analyze information (synthesis analysis), whereas shallow processors prefer structured fact retention learning styles and are better suited for elaborative processing.327 The main functions of these four specific learning styles are as follows:

NameFunction
Synthesis analysis:processing information, forming categories, and organizing them into hierarchies. This is the only one of the learning styles that has explained a significant impact on academic performance.328
Methodical study:methodical behavior while completing academic assignments
Fact retention:focusing on the actual result instead of understanding the logic behind something
Elaborative processing:connecting and applying new ideas to existing knowledge

Openness has been linked to learning styles that often lead to academic success and higher grades like synthesis analysis and methodical study. Because conscientiousness and openness have been shown to predict all four learning styles, it suggests that individuals who possess characteristics like discipline, determination, and curiosity are more likely to engage in all of the above learning styles.329

According to the research carried out by Komarraju, Karau, Schmeck & Avdic (2011), conscientiousness and agreeableness are positively related with all four learning styles, whereas neuroticism was negatively related with those four. Furthermore, extraversion and openness were only positively related to elaborative processing, and openness itself correlated with higher academic achievement.330

In addition, a previous study by psychologist Mikael Jensen has shown relationships between the Big Five personality traits, learning, and academic achievement. According to Jensen, all personality traits, except neuroticism, are associated with learning goals and motivation. Openness and conscientiousness influence individuals to learn to a high degree unrecognized, while extraversion and agreeableness have similar effects.331 Conscientiousness and neuroticism also influence individuals to perform well in front of others for a sense of credit and reward, while agreeableness forces individuals to avoid this strategy of learning.332 Jensen's study concludes that individuals who score high on the agreeableness trait will likely learn just to perform well in front of others.333

Besides openness, all Big Five personality traits helped predict the educational identity of students. Based on these findings, scientists are beginning to see that the Big Five traits might have a large influence of on academic motivation that leads to predicting a student's academic performance.334

Some authors suggested that Big Five personality traits combined with learning styles can help predict some variations in the academic performance and the academic motivation of an individual which can then influence their academic achievements.335 This may be seen because individual differences in personality represent stable approaches to information processing. For instance, conscientiousness has consistently emerged as a stable predictor of success in exam performance, largely because conscientious students experience fewer study delays.336 Conscientiousness shows a positive association with the four learning styles because students with high levels of conscientiousness develop focused learning strategies and appear to be more disciplined and achievement-oriented.

Personality and learning styles are both likely to play significant roles in influencing academic achievement. College students (308 undergraduates) completed the Five Factor Inventory and the Inventory of Learning Processes and reported their grade point average. Two of the Big Five traits, conscientiousness and agreeableness, were positively related with all four learning styles (synthesis analysis, methodical study, fact retention, and elaborative processing), whereas neuroticism was negatively related with all four learning styles. In addition, extraversion and openness were positively related with elaborative processing. The Big Five together explained 14% of the variance in grade point average (GPA), and learning styles explained an additional 3%, suggesting that both personality traits and learning styles contribute to academic performance. Further, the relationship between openness and GPA was mediated by reflective learning styles (synthesis-analysis and elaborative processing). These latter results suggest that being intellectually curious fully enhances academic performance when students combine this scholarly interest with thoughtful information processing. Implications of these results are discussed in the context of teaching techniques and curriculum design.

— M Komarraju337

Distance Learning

When the relationship between the five-factor personality traits and academic achievement in distance education settings was examined in brief, the openness personality trait was found to be the most important variable that has a positive relationship with academic achievement in distance education environments. In addition, it was found that self-discipline, extraversion, and adaptability personality traits are generally in a positive relationship with academic achievement. The most important personality trait that has a negative relationship with academic achievement has emerged as neuroticism. The results generally show that individuals who are organized, planned, determined, who are oriented to new ideas and independent thinking have increased success in distance education environments. On the other hand, it can be said that individuals with anxiety and stress tendencies generally have lower academic success.338339340

Employment

Occupation and personality fit

Researchers have long suggested that work is more likely to be fulfilling to the individual and beneficial to society when there is alignment between the person and their occupation.341 For instance, software programmers and scientists often rank high on Openness to experience and tend to be intellectually curious, think in symbols and abstractions, and find repetition boring.342 Psychologists and sociologists rank higher on Agreeableness and Openness than economists and jurists.343

Work success

It is believed that the Big Five traits are predictors of future performance outcomes to varying degrees. Specific facets of the Big Five traits are also thought to be indicators of success in the workplace, and each individual facet can give a more precise indication as to the nature of a person. Different traits' facets are needed for different occupations. Various facets of the Big Five traits can predict the success of people in different environments. The estimated levels of an individual's success in jobs that require public speaking versus one-on-one interactions will differ according to whether that person has particular traits' facets.344

Job outcome measures include job and training proficiency and personnel data.345 However, research demonstrating such prediction has been criticized, in part because of the apparently low correlation coefficients characterizing the relationship between personality and job performance. In a 2007 article states: "The problem with personality tests is ... that the validity of personality measures as predictors of job performance is often disappointingly low. The argument for using personality tests to predict performance does not strike me as convincing in the first place."346

Such criticisms were put forward by Walter Mischel,347 whose publication caused a two-decades' long crisis in personality psychometrics. However, later work demonstrated that the correlations obtained by psychometric personality researchers were actually very respectable by comparative standards,348 and that the economic value of even incremental increases in prediction accuracy was exceptionally large, given the vast difference in performance by those who occupy complex job positions.349

Research has suggested that individuals who are considered leaders typically exhibit lower amounts of neurotic traits, maintain higher levels of openness, balanced levels of conscientiousness, and balanced levels of extraversion.350351352 Further studies have linked professional burnout to neuroticism, and extraversion to enduring positive work experience.353 Studies have linked national innovation, leadership, and ideation to openness to experience and conscientiousness.354 Occupational self-efficacy has also been shown to be positively correlated with conscientiousness and negatively correlated with neuroticism.355 Some research has also suggested that the conscientiousness of a supervisor is positively associated with an employee's perception of abusive supervision.356 Others have suggested that low agreeableness and high neuroticism are traits more related to abusive supervision.357

Openness is positively related to proactivity at the individual and the organizational levels and is negatively related to team and organizational proficiency. These effects were found to be completely independent of one another. This is also counter-conscientious and has a negative correlation to Conscientiousness.358

Agreeableness is negatively related to individual task proactivity. Typically this is associated with lower career success and being less able to cope with conflict. However there are benefits to the Agreeableness personality trait including higher subjective well-being; more positive interpersonal interactions and helping behavior; lower conflict; lower deviance and turnover.359 Furthermore, attributes related to Agreeableness are important for workforce readiness for a variety of occupations and performance criteria.360 Research has suggested that those who are high in agreeableness are not as successful in accumulating income.361

Extraversion results in greater leadership emergence and effectiveness; as well as higher job and life satisfaction. However extraversion can lead to more impulsive behaviors, more accidents and lower performance in certain jobs.362

Conscientiousness is highly predictive of job performance in general,363 and is positively related to all forms of work role performance, including job performance and job satisfaction, greater leadership effectiveness, lower turnover and deviant behaviors. However this personality trait is associated with reduced adaptability, lower learning in initial stages of skill acquisition and more interpersonally abrasiveness, when also low in agreeableness.364 It is also not the case that more or extreme conscientiousness is always necessarily better as there does appear to be a link between conscientiousness and obsessive-compulsive personality disorder (OCPD). Selecting employees for a moderate level of conscientiousness may actually provide the best occupational outcome.365

Neuroticism is negatively related to all forms of work role performance. This increases the chance of engaging in risky behaviors.366367

Two theories have been integrated in an attempt to account for these differences in work role performance. Trait activation theory posits that within a person trait levels predict future behavior, that trait levels differ between people, and that work-related cues activate traits which leads to work relevant behaviors. Role theory suggests that role senders provide cues to elicit desired behaviors. In this context, role senders provide workers with cues for expected behaviors, which in turn activates personality traits and work relevant behaviors. In essence, expectations of the role sender lead to different behavioral outcomes depending on the trait levels of individual workers, and because people differ in trait levels, responses to these cues will not be universal.368

Remote Work/Telework

As of 2020, remote work has become more and more prevalent as brought on by the COVID-19 pandemic. However, research has shown that the Big Five personality traits still influence remote work. Gavoille and Hazans have found that conscientiousness (β=0.06) and openness to experience are both positively correlated with willingness to work and worker productivity within a remote setting, with openness to experience being less significant (β=0.021). This is then contrasted with extraversion (β=-0.038), which negatively correlates with Willingness to work and openness. Another conclusion that was found is that gender did not play a role in the difference between conscientiousness and extraversion, and willingness to work from home.369 Similarly, Wright investigated the influence of Big Five on the soft skills in the remote workplace, such as effort and cooperation. She delineated soft skills into two different groups, Task Performance and Contextual Performance, with each having three subgroups. Task Performance was more aligned with specific job responsibilities and handling cognitive tasks associated with their job, and the three subgroups were Job Knowledge, Organizational Skills, and Efficiency. Wright found that Job Knowledge did not correlate with any Big Five traits, Organizational Skill is only significantly correlated with Conscientiousness (T=7.952, P=.001), and Efficiency is significantly correlated with Conscientiousness (T=3.8, P=.001), and Neuroticism(T=-2.6, P=.008), which it is a negative correlation. Contextual Performance is concerned with non-job core requirements, such as perceived effort and job cooperation, with the subgroups being Persistent Effort, Cooperation, and Organizational Conscientiousness. Wright found that Persistent Effort is positively correlated with Openness(t=2.4, P=.014) and Conscientiousness (T=3.1, P=.002), and negatively correlated with Neuroticism (T=-3.2, P=.001). Cooperation was positively correlated with Extraversion (t=2.6, P=.009) and Conscientiousness (t=2.82, P=.005), as well as Organizational Conscientiousness was positively correlated with Agreeableness (t=4.059, P<.001) and Conscientiousness (t=4.511, P<.001)370

On another tack, scientists wanted to discover if the Big Five has any effect on remote worker burnout, and the effect that different Big Five traits have on worker health and engagement. Olsen et al found that when remote work days are increased, individuals high in extraversion start to struggle with work engagement (β=-.094, P<.03), and individuals with higher neuroticism are more likely to have poorer health (p= -.23), work engagement (p=-.18), and an increase in sick leaves(p=.38).371 However, Olsen found that conscientiousness, coupled with an increase in remote work days, can lead to a decrease in general health, contrary to all of the benefits it has listed above. Similarly, Para et al. found that individuals with higher Neuroticism (β=.138, p<.05) also tend to have higher Remote Work Exhaustion (RWE). They also found that conscientiousness(β=-.336, p<.001) and agreeableness (β=-.267, p<.001) were negatively correlated with RWE, meaning that they were more resilient against RWE over large spans of remote work days.372 The author attributed conscientious individuals to being hard workers and dependable, while agreeableness was attributed to the situation the study was completed under, which was the at-home quarantine due to COVID-19, stating individuals with high agreeableness did well with the forced contact due to quarantine, which transferred over to their work.

Romantic relationships

Various researchers have explored the association of Big Five and romantic relationships in terms of relationship satisfaction.373374375 A meta-analysis showed that there was a higher level of marital satisfaction if their spouse showed lower levels in neuroticism (.22), but higher levels in agreeableness (.15) and conscientiousness(.12). There was only a weak correlation, but it was the same level of satisfaction for both genders. Much like the previous meta-analysis, a study on self-reported big five traits showed that those with higher levels of agreeableness, emotional stability, conscientiousness, and extraversion had higher levels of marital satisfaction(.20). That same study found that there was little to no difference in marital satisfaction if the two partners had similar or different levels of trait personality.376

O’Brien and colleagues377 examined the association of Big Five and romantic relationships by investigating participants’ commitment levels. The three levels of commitment are affective commitment (emotional attachment), continuance commitment (financial considerations), and normative commitment (the ethical and moral responsibilities). The commitment levels were based on the taxonomy of organizational commitment378 and the conceptual model of marital commitment of Johnson379 and Johnson et al.380 122 Individuals currently in a committed relationship responded to a 50-item personality questionnaire from the International Personality Item Pool (IPIP, 2006), and a questionnaire on commitment modified from Allen.381 The key findings showed that participants high in Extraversion reported high levels of affective commitment; participants high on Extraversion were higher on Openness to Experience and affective commitment. Conscientiousness demonstrated a negative relationship with continuance commitment. While Extraversion and Agreeableness exhibited a positive correlation with each other, no significant relationships were found between Agreeableness and any of the commitment measures. The findings indicated gender differences in that women with lower levels of Openness to Experience were often paired with partners who scored higher in Extraversion.  Men who exhibited strong affective commitment were more likely to be in relationships with women high in Conscientiousness. Additionally, women whose partners showed high affective commitment tended to be higher in both Conscientiousness and Emotional Stability.

Asselmann and Sprecht382 examined the association of Big Five (BFI-S) and romantic relationships through major life events across years in 2005, 2009, 2013, and 2017 with a sample of 49,932 participants in Germany. Those major life events are (1) moving in with a partner, (2) getting married, (3) getting separated, and (4) getting divorced. Researchers also examined whether the Big Five personality traits play a significant role in romantic relationships. Along the spectrum of a person’s life satisfaction, marital satisfaction (one of romantic relationships) is shown to be stronger than job satisfaction, health satisfaction, and social satisfaction.383 The key findings from Asselmann and Sprecht showed that more extraverted individuals were more likely to move in with a partner. Less agreeable and less emotionally stable women were more likely to move in with a partner. Men were more extraverted in the years before moving in and became gradually more open and more conscientious after moving in. Less agreeable men were more likely to get married. Individuals who got married became less open in the first three years after the marriage. Women became more extraverted after being separated. Men with lower emotional stability and women who were both less emotionally stable and more extraverted were more prone to experiencing relationship breakups. Individuals who got divorced were less agreeable in the years before the divorce. Personality may change after specific events. For example, both men and women who experienced separation or divorce became less emotionally stable in the following years. The results implicated that total agreeableness was not a guarantee for long-lasting romantic relationships, as less agreeable individuals were more likely to experience both positive and negative major romantic events.384 Getting into a long-term romantic relationship can kick-start personality development in young adults ages 20–30 as they are faced with new social situations and expectations. For instance, high levels of trait neuroticism at the beginning of relationships can be seen decreasing over 8 years once the relationship has begun, as well as other Big Five personality traits, such as Conscientiousness and Agreeableness, can be seen increasing in long-term relationships.385

Political identification

The Big Five Personality Model also has applications in the study of political psychology. Studies have been finding links between the big five personality traits and political identification. It has been found by several studies that individuals who score high in Conscientiousness are more likely to possess a right-wing political identification.386387388 On the opposite end of the spectrum, a strong correlation was identified between high scores in Openness to Experience and a left-leaning ideology.389390391 While the traits of agreeableness, extraversion, and neuroticism have not been consistently linked to either conservative or liberal ideology, with studies producing mixed results, such traits are promising when analyzing the strength of an individual's party identification.392393 However, correlations between the Big Five and political beliefs, while present, tend to be small, with one study finding correlations ranged from 0.14 to 0.24.394

Scope of predictive power

The predictive effects of the Big Five personality traits relate mostly to social functioning and rules-driven behavior and are not very specific for prediction of particular aspects of behavior. For example, it was noted by all temperament researchers that high neuroticism precedes the development of all common mental disorders395 and is not associated with personality.396 Further evidence is required to fully uncover the nature and differences between personality traits, temperament and life outcomes. Social and contextual parameters also play a role in outcomes and the interaction between the two is not yet fully understood.397

Religiosity

Though the effect sizes are small: Of the Big Five personality traits high Agreeableness, Conscientiousness and Extraversion relate to general religiosity, while Openness relate negatively to religious fundamentalism and positively to spirituality. High Neuroticism may be related to extrinsic religiosity, whereas intrinsic religiosity and spirituality reflect Emotional Stability.398

Measurements

Several measures of the Big Five exist:

  • International Personality Item Pool (IPIP)399
  • NEO-PI-R
  • The Ten-Item Personality Inventory (TIPI) and the Five Item Personality Inventory (FIPI) are very abbreviated rating forms of the Big Five personality traits.400
  • Self-descriptive sentence questionnaires401
  • Lexical questionnaires402
  • Self-report questionnaires403
  • Relative-scored Big 5 measure404

The most frequently used measures of the Big Five comprise either items that are self-descriptive sentences405 or, in the case of lexical measures, items that are single adjectives.406 Due to the length of sentence-based and some lexical measures, short forms have been developed and validated for use in applied research settings where questionnaire space and respondent time are limited, such as the 40-item balanced International English Big-Five Mini-Markers407 or a very brief (10 item) measure of the Big Five domains.408 Research has suggested that some methodologies in administering personality tests are inadequate in length and provide insufficient detail to truly evaluate personality. Usually, longer, more detailed questions will give a more accurate portrayal of personality.409 At the same time, shorter questionnaires may be sufficient to get a reasonable estimate of Big Five personality scores when questions are carefully selected and statistical imputation is used.410 The five factor structure has been replicated in peer reports.411 However, many of the substantive findings rely on self-reports.

Much of the evidence on the measures of the Big 5 relies on self-report questionnaires, which makes self-report bias and falsification of responses difficult to deal with and account for.412 It has been argued that the Big Five tests do not create an accurate personality profile because the responses given on these tests are not true in all cases and can be falsified.413 For example, questionnaires are answered by potential employees who might choose answers that paint them in the best light.414

Research suggests that a relative-scored Big Five measure in which respondents had to make repeated choices between equally desirable personality descriptors may be a potential alternative to traditional Big Five measures in accurately assessing personality traits, especially when lying or biased responding is present.415 When compared with a traditional Big Five measure for its ability to predict GPA and creative achievement under both normal and "fake good"-bias response conditions, the relative-scored measure significantly and consistently predicted these outcomes under both conditions; however, the Likert questionnaire lost its predictive ability in the faking condition. Thus, the relative-scored measure proved to be less affected by biased responding than the Likert measure of the Big Five.

Andrew H. Schwartz analyzed 700 million words, phrases, and topic instances collected from the Facebook messages of 75,000 volunteers, who also took standard personality tests, and found striking variations in language with personality, gender, and age.416

See also

References

  1. Roccas, Sonia; Sagiv, Lilach; Schwartz, Shalom H.; Knafo, Ariel (2002). &quot;The Big Five Personality Factors and Personal Values&quot;. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 28 (6): 789&#8211;801. doi:10.1177/0146167202289008. S2CID&nbsp;144611052. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/0146167202289008

  2. Goldberg LR (January 1993). &quot;The structure of phenotypic personality traits&quot;. American Psychologist. 48 (1): 26&#8211;34. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.48.1.26. PMID&nbsp;8427480. S2CID&nbsp;20595956. https://archive.org/details/sim_american-psychologist_1993-01_48_1/page/26

  3. Costa PT, McCrae RR (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) manual. Odessa, Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources. /wiki/Odessa,_Florida

  4. Matthews G, Deary IJ, Whiteman MC (2003). Personality Traits (PDF) (2nd&nbsp;ed.). Cambridge University Press. ISBN&nbsp;978-0-521-83107-9. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2014-12-05. 978-0-521-83107-9

  5. Cite error: The named reference :18 was invoked but never defined (see the help page). /wiki/Help:Cite_errors/Cite_error_references_no_text

  6. Digman JM (1990). &quot;Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model&quot;. Annual Review of Psychology. 41: 417&#8211;40. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.002221. https://archive.org/details/sim_annual-review-of-psychology_1990_41/page/417

  7. Poropat AE (March 2009). &quot;A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance&quot;. Psychological Bulletin. 135 (2): 322&#8211;38. doi:10.1037/a0014996. hdl:10072/30324. PMID&nbsp;19254083. https://archive.org/details/sim_psychological-bulletin_2009-03_135_2/page/322

  8. Digman JM (1990). &quot;Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model&quot;. Annual Review of Psychology. 41: 417&#8211;40. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.002221. https://archive.org/details/sim_annual-review-of-psychology_1990_41/page/417

  9. Shrout PE, Fiske ST (1995). Personality research, methods, and theory. Psychology Press.

  10. Allport GW, Odbert HS (1936). &quot;Trait names: A psycholexical study&quot;. Psychological Monographs. 47: 211. doi:10.1037/h0093360. https://archive.org/details/sim_psychological-monographs_1936_47_212/page/211

  11. Bagby RM, Marshall MB, Georgiades S (February 2005). &quot;Dimensional personality traits and the prediction of DSM-IV personality disorder symptom counts in a nonclinical sample&quot;. Journal of Personality Disorders. 19 (1): 53&#8211;67. doi:10.1521/pedi.19.1.53.62180. PMID&nbsp;15899720. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-disorders_2005-02_19_1/page/53

  12. Tupes EC, Christal RE (1961). &quot;Recurrent personality factors based on trait ratings&quot;. USAF ASD Tech. Rep. 60 (61&#8211;97): 225&#8211;51. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00973.x. PMID&nbsp;1635043. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  13. Norman WT (June 1963). &quot;Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality attributes: replicated factors structure in peer nomination personality ratings&quot;. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 66 (6): 574&#8211;83. doi:10.1037/h0040291. PMID&nbsp;13938947. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  14. Goldberg LR (January 1993). &quot;The structure of phenotypic personality traits&quot;. American Psychologist. 48 (1): 26&#8211;34. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.48.1.26. PMID&nbsp;8427480. S2CID&nbsp;20595956. https://archive.org/details/sim_american-psychologist_1993-01_48_1/page/26

  15. O'Connor BP (June 2002). &quot;A quantitative review of the comprehensiveness of the five-factor model in relation to popular personality inventories&quot;. Assessment. 9 (2): 188&#8211;203. doi:10.1177/1073191102092010. PMID&nbsp;12066834. S2CID&nbsp;145580837. https://archive.org/details/sim_assessment_2002-06_9_2/page/188

  16. Goldberg LR (1982). &quot;From Ace to Zombie: Some explorations in the language of personality&quot;. In Spielberger CD, Butcher JN (eds.). Advances in personality assessment. Vol.&nbsp;1. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. pp.&nbsp;201&#8211;34.

  17. Norman WT, Goldberg LR (1966). &quot;Raters, ratees, and randomness in personality structure&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 4 (6): 681&#8211;91. doi:10.1037/h0024002. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_1966-12_4_6/page/681

  18. Peabody D, Goldberg LR (September 1989). &quot;Some determinants of factor structures from personality-trait descriptors&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 57 (3): 552&#8211;67. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.3.552. PMID&nbsp;2778639. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_1989-09_57_3/page/552

  19. Saucier G, Goldberg LR (1996). &quot;The language of personality: Lexical perspectives on the five-factor model&quot;. In Wiggins JS (ed.). The five-factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives. New York: Guilford.[page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  20. Digman JM (June 1989). &quot;Five robust trait dimensions: development, stability, and utility&quot;. Journal of Personality. 57 (2): 195&#8211;214. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1989.tb00480.x. PMID&nbsp;2671337. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  21. Bagby RM, Marshall MB, Georgiades S (February 2005). &quot;Dimensional personality traits and the prediction of DSM-IV personality disorder symptom counts in a nonclinical sample&quot;. Journal of Personality Disorders. 19 (1): 53&#8211;67. doi:10.1521/pedi.19.1.53.62180. PMID&nbsp;15899720. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-disorders_2005-02_19_1/page/53

  22. Karson S, O'Dell JW (1976). A guide to the clinical use of the 16PF (Report). Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality &amp; Ability Testing.

  23. Krug SE, Johns EF (1986). &quot;A large scale cross-validation of second-order personality structure defined by the 16PF&quot;. Psychological Reports. 59 (2): 683&#8211;93. doi:10.2466/pr0.1986.59.2.683. S2CID&nbsp;145610003. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  24. Cattell HE, Mead AD (2007). &quot;The 16 Personality Factor Questionnaire (16PF)&quot;. In Boyle GJ, Matthews G, Saklofske DH (eds.). Handbook of personality theory and testing, Volume 2: Personality measurement and assessment. London: Sage.[page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  25. Costa PT, McCrae RR (September 1976). &quot;Age differences in personality structure: a cluster analytic approach&quot;. Journal of Gerontology. 31 (5): 564&#8211;70. doi:10.1093/geronj/31.5.564. PMID&nbsp;950450. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-gerontology_1976-09_31_5/page/564

  26. Costa PT, McCrae RR (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

  27. McCrae RR, Costa PT (January 1987). &quot;Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 52 (1): 81&#8211;90. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.1.81. PMID&nbsp;3820081. S2CID&nbsp;7893185. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_1987-01_52_1/page/81

  28. McCrae RR, John OP (June 1992). &quot;An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications&quot;. Journal of Personality. 60 (2): 175&#8211;215. CiteSeerX&nbsp;10.1.1.470.4858. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x. PMID&nbsp;1635039. S2CID&nbsp;10596836. /wiki/CiteSeerX_(identifier)

  29. &quot;International Personality Item Pool (IPIP)&quot;. The Society for Judgment and Decision Making. http://sjdm.org/dmidi/International_Personality_Item_Pool.html

  30. Goldberg LR, Johnson JA, Eber HW, Hogan R, Ashton MC, Cloninger CR, Gough HG (February 2006). &quot;The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures&quot;. Journal of Research in Personality. 40 (1): 84&#8211;96. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007. S2CID&nbsp;13274640. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  31. Conn S, Rieke M (1994). The 16PF Fifth Edition technical manual. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality &amp; Ability Testing.

  32. Cattell HE (1996). &quot;The original big five: A historical perspective&quot;. European Review of Applied Psychology. 46: 5&#8211;14.

  33. Grucza RA, Goldberg LR (October 2007). &quot;The comparative validity of 11 modern personality inventories: predictions of behavioral acts, informant reports, and clinical indicators&quot;. Journal of Personality Assessment. 89 (2): 167&#8211;87. doi:10.1080/00223890701468568. PMID&nbsp;17764394. S2CID&nbsp;42394327. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-assessment_2007-10_89_2/page/167

  34. Mershon B, Gorsuch RL (1988). &quot;Number of factors in the personality sphere: does increase in factors increase predictability of real-life criteria?&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 55 (4): 675&#8211;80. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.55.4.675. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  35. Paunonen SV, Ashton MS (2001). &quot;Big Five factors and facets and the prediction of behavior&quot;. Journal of Personality &amp; Social Psychology. 81 (3): 524&#8211;39. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.3.524. PMID&nbsp;11554651. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  36. Deyoung, C. G.; Quilty, L. C.; Peterson, J. B. (2007). &quot;Between Facets and Domains: 10 Aspects of the Big Five&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 93 (5): 880&#8211;896. CiteSeerX&nbsp;10.1.1.513.2517. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880. PMID&nbsp;17983306. S2CID&nbsp;8261816. /wiki/CiteSeerX_(identifier)

  37. Deyoung, C. G.; Quilty, L. C.; Peterson, J. B. (2007). &quot;Between Facets and Domains: 10 Aspects of the Big Five&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 93 (5): 880&#8211;896. CiteSeerX&nbsp;10.1.1.513.2517. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880. PMID&nbsp;17983306. S2CID&nbsp;8261816. /wiki/CiteSeerX_(identifier)

  38. Shrout PE, Fiske ST (1995). Personality research, methods, and theory. Psychology Press.

  39. Allport GW, Odbert HS (1936). &quot;Trait names: A psycholexical study&quot;. Psychological Monographs. 47: 211. doi:10.1037/h0093360. https://archive.org/details/sim_psychological-monographs_1936_47_1

  40. Cattell, Raymond B. (October 1943). &quot;The description of personality: basic traits resolved into clusters&quot;. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 38 (4): 476&#8211;506. doi:10.1037/h0054116. ISSN&nbsp;0096-851X. http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/h0054116

  41. Cattell, Raymond B. (1945). &quot;The Description of Personality: Principles and Findings in a Factor Analysis&quot;. The American Journal of Psychology. 58 (1): 69&#8211;90. doi:10.2307/1417576. JSTOR&nbsp;1417576. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  42. Cattell, Raymond B. (1947-09-01). &quot;Confirmation and clarification of primary personality factors&quot;. Psychometrika. 12 (3): 197&#8211;220. doi:10.1007/BF02289253. ISSN&nbsp;1860-0980. PMID&nbsp;20260610. S2CID&nbsp;28667497. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  43. Cattell, Raymond B. (July 1948). &quot;The primary personality factors in women compared with those in men&quot;. British Journal of Statistical Psychology. 1 (2): 114&#8211;130. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8317.1948.tb00231.x. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  44. h.j. eysenck (1950). dimensions of personality. Internet Archive. routledge &amp; kegan paul limited. http://archive.org/details/dimensionsofpers0000hjey_e0a7

  45. Fiske, Donald W. (July 1949). &quot;Consistency of the factorial structures of personality ratings from different sources&quot;. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 44 (3): 329&#8211;344. doi:10.1037/h0057198. hdl:2027.42/179031. ISSN&nbsp;0096-851X. PMID&nbsp;18146776. http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/h0057198

  46. Cattell, R.B. (1973). Personality and mood by questionnaire. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.[page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  47. Cattell, R.B. (1957). Personality and motivation structure and measurement. New York: World Book

  48. Cattell, H. B. (1989). &quot;The 16PF: Personality In Depth.&quot; Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing, Inc.

  49. Linn, Robert L. (1996). &quot;In Memoriam: Maurice M. Tatsuoka (1922-1996)&quot;. Journal of Educational Measurement. 33 (2): 125&#8211;127. doi:10.1111/j.1745-3984.1996.tb00484.x. ISSN&nbsp;0022-0655. JSTOR&nbsp;1435178. https://www.jstor.org/stable/1435178

  50. &quot;Wayback Machine&quot;. www.legis.ga.gov. https://www.legis.ga.gov/api/legislation/document/20152016/161898

  51. French, John W. (March 1953). &quot;The Description of Personality Measurements in Terms of Rotated Factors&quot;. Eric. Institute of Educational Sciences. ERIC&nbsp;ED079418. /wiki/ERIC_(identifier)

  52. Tupes, Ernest C. (1957). &quot;Relationships between behavior trait ratings by peers and later officer performance of USAF Officer Candidate School graduates&quot;. PsycEXTRA Dataset. doi:10.1037/e522552009-001. Retrieved 2023-02-10. https://doi.apa.org/get-pe-doi.cfm?doi=10.1037/e522552009-001

  53. AFPTRC-TN. Air Force Personnel &amp; Training Research Center, Lackland Air Force Base. 1957. https://books.google.com/books?id=Wd6dpewPPkMC

  54. &quot;A Memorium to Raymond E. Christal&quot;. PsycEXTRA Dataset. 1995. doi:10.1037/e568692011-006. Retrieved 2023-02-10. https://psycnet.apa.org/get-pe-doi.cfm?doi=10.1037/e568692011-006

  55. Tupes, Ernest C.; Christal, Raymond C. (1958). Stability of Personality Trait Rating Factors Obtained Under Diverse Conditions. Personnel Laboratory, Wright Air Development Center, Air Research and Development Command, United States Air Force. https://books.google.com/books?id=S4Ul-sQV7_AC

  56. Christal, Raymond E. (June 1992). &quot;Author's Note on &quot;Recurrent Personality Factors Based on Trait Ratings&quot;&quot;. Journal of Personality. 60 (2): 221&#8211;224. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00972.x. ISSN&nbsp;0022-3506. PMID&nbsp;1635042. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00972.x

  57. Tupes, Ernest C.; Christal, Raymond C. (1958). Stability of Personality Trait Rating Factors Obtained Under Diverse Conditions. Personnel Laboratory, Wright Air Development Center, Air Research and Development Command, United States Air Force. https://books.google.com/books?id=S4Ul-sQV7_AC

  58. Tupes, Ernest C.; Christal, Raymond E. (May 1961). &quot;Recurrent Personality Factors Based on Trait Ratings&quot;. Aeronautical Systems Division Technical Reports and Technical Notes. 26 (2). https://books.google.com/books?id=HlGIjgEACAAJ

  59. Tupes EC, Christal RE (1961). &quot;Recurrent personality factors based on trait ratings&quot;. USAF ASD Tech. Rep. 60 (61&#8211;97): 225&#8211;51. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00973.x. PMID&nbsp;1635043. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  60. Goldberg, Lewis R. (1998-12-01). &quot;Warren T. Norman (1930&#8211;1998): An Appreciation&quot;. Journal of Research in Personality. 32 (4): 391&#8211;396. doi:10.1006/jrpe.1998.2224. ISSN&nbsp;0092-6566. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0092656698922243

  61. Norman WT (June 1963). &quot;Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality attributes: replicated factors structure in peer nomination personality ratings&quot;. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 66 (6): 574&#8211;83. doi:10.1037/h0040291. PMID&nbsp;13938947. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  62. &quot;Finding Scales to Measure Particular Personality Constructs&quot;. ipip.ori.org. Retrieved 2023-02-11. https://ipip.ori.org/Finding_Scales_to_Measure_Particular_Constructs.htm

  63. &quot;Richard Miller&quot; (PDF). https://people.wku.edu/richard.miller/520%2016PF%20Cattell%20and%20Mead.pdf

  64. &quot;APA PsycNet&quot;. psycnet.apa.org. Retrieved 2023-02-12. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1998-11586-001

  65. Costa, Paul T.; McCrae, Robert R. (1978). &quot;Objective Personality Assessment&quot;. In Storandt, Martha; Siegler, Ilene C.; Elias, Merrill F. (eds.). The Clinical Psychology of Aging. Boston, MA: Springer US. pp.&nbsp;119&#8211;143. doi:10.1007/978-1-4684-3342-5_5. ISBN&nbsp;978-1-4684-3342-5. 978-1-4684-3342-5

  66. McCrae, Robert R.; Costa, Paul T. (December 1980). &quot;Openness to experience and ego level in Loevinger's Sentence Completion Test: Dispositional contributions to developmental models of personality&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 39 (6): 1179&#8211;1190. doi:10.1037/h0077727. ISSN&nbsp;1939-1315. http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/h0077727

  67. Saville, Peter (1978). A critical analysis of Cattell's model of personality (Thesis thesis). Brunel University School of Sport and Education PhD Theses. http://bura.brunel.ac.uk/handle/2438/7732

  68. Goldberg LR (May 1980). Some ruminations about the structure of individual differences: Developing a common lexicon for the major characteristics of human personality. Symposium presentation at the meeting of the Western Psychological Association (Report). Honolulu, HI.[page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  69. Digman, John M.; Takemoto-Chock, Naomi K. (1981-04-01). &quot;Factors In The Natural Language Of Personality: Re-Analysis, Comparison, And Interpretation Of Six Major Studies&quot;. Multivariate Behavioral Research. 16 (2): 149&#8211;170. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr1602_2. ISSN&nbsp;0027-3171. PMID&nbsp;26825420. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  70. &quot;Society, August 2012&quot;. BPS. Retrieved 2023-02-11. https://www.bps.org.uk/psychologist/society-august-2012

  71. Stanton, N.A.; Mathews, G.; Graham, N.C.; Brimelow, C. (1991-01-01). &quot;The Opq and the Big Five&quot;. Journal of Managerial Psychology. 6 (1): 25&#8211;27. doi:10.1108/02683949110140750. ISSN&nbsp;0268-3946. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  72. Boyle GJ, Stankov L, Cattell RB (1995). &quot;Measurement and statistical models in the study of personality and intelligence&quot;. In Saklofske DH, Zeidner M (eds.). International Handbook of Personality and Intelligence. pp.&nbsp;417&#8211;46.[ISBN&nbsp;missing] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  73. Epstein S, O'Brien EJ (November 1985). &quot;The person-situation debate in historical and current perspective&quot;. Psychological Bulletin. 98 (3): 513&#8211;37. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.98.3.513. PMID&nbsp;4080897. https://archive.org/details/sim_psychological-bulletin_1985-11_98_3/page/513

  74. Kenrick DT, Funder DC (January 1988). &quot;Profiting from controversy. Lessons from the person-situation debate&quot;. The American Psychologist. 43 (1): 23&#8211;34. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.43.1.23. PMID&nbsp;3279875. https://archive.org/details/sim_american-psychologist_1988-01_43_1/page/23

  75. &quot;Finding Scales to Measure Particular Personality Constructs&quot;. ipip.ori.org. Retrieved 2023-02-11. https://ipip.ori.org/Finding_Scales_to_Measure_Particular_Constructs.htm

  76. &quot;History of the IPIP&quot;. ipip.ori.org. Retrieved 2023-02-11. https://ipip.ori.org/HistoryOfTheIPIP.htm

  77. Goldberg, Lewis R.; Johnson, John A.; Eber, Herbert W.; Hogan, Robert; Ashton, Michael C.; Cloninger, C. Robert; Gough, Harrison G. (2006). &quot;The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures&quot; (PDF). Journal of Research in Personality. 40: 84&#8211;96. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007. S2CID&nbsp;13274640 &#8211; via Elsevier. https://ipip.ori.org/Goldberg_etal_2006_IPIP_JRP.pdf

  78. &quot;The HEXACO Personality Inventory - Revised&quot;. hexaco.org. Retrieved 2023-02-11. https://hexaco.org/history

  79. Deyoung, C. G.; Quilty, L. C.; Peterson, J. B. (2007). &quot;Between Facets and Domains: 10 Aspects of the Big Five&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 93 (5): 880&#8211;896. CiteSeerX&nbsp;10.1.1.513.2517. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880. PMID&nbsp;17983306. S2CID&nbsp;8261816. /wiki/CiteSeerX_(identifier)

  80. Jang, KL (2002). &quot;The revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI-R)&quot;. The SAGE Handbook of Personality Theory and Assessment. 2: 223&#8211;257 &#8211; via ResearchGate. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/285086638

  81. Lucas RE, Donnellan MB (2009). &quot;If the person-situation debate is really over, why does it still generate so much negative affect?&quot;. Journal of Research in Personality. 43 (3): 146&#8211;49. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2009.02.009. /wiki/M._Brent_Donnellan

  82. Alexander, Nix (2017-03-03). &quot;From Mad Men to Math Men&quot;. Freud Online. Retrieved 2022-10-23. https://freud.online/articles/from-mad-men-to-math-men

  83. &quot;About Us&quot;. Cambridge Analytica. Archived from the original on 16 February 2016. Retrieved 27 December 2015. https://web.archive.org/web/20160216023554/https://cambridgeanalytica.org/about

  84. Sellers FS (19 October 2015). &quot;Cruz campaign paid $750,000 to 'psychographic profiling' company&quot;. The Washington Post. Retrieved 7 February 2016. https://www.washingtonpost.com/politics/cruz-campaign-paid-750000-to-psychographic-profiling-company/2015/10/19/6c83e508-743f-11e5-9cbb-790369643cf9_story.html

  85. Goldberg LR (January 1993). &quot;The structure of phenotypic personality traits&quot;. American Psychologist. 48 (1): 26&#8211;34. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.48.1.26. PMID&nbsp;8427480. S2CID&nbsp;20595956. https://archive.org/details/sim_american-psychologist_1993-01_48_1/page/26

  86. Costa PT, McCrae RR (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) manual. Odessa, Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources. /wiki/Odessa,_Florida

  87. Matthews G, Deary IJ, Whiteman MC (2003). Personality Traits (PDF) (2nd&nbsp;ed.). Cambridge University Press. ISBN&nbsp;978-0-521-83107-9. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2014-12-05. 978-0-521-83107-9

  88. De Bolle M, Beyers W, De Clercq B, De Fruyt F (November 2012). &quot;General personality and psychopathology in referred and nonreferred children and adolescents: an investigation of continuity, pathoplasty, and complication models&quot;. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 121 (4): 958&#8211;70. doi:10.1037/a0027742. PMID&nbsp;22448741. S2CID&nbsp;33228527. https://biblio.ugent.be/publication/2117776/file/6770996

  89. Ambridge B (2014). Psy-Q: You know your IQ &#8211; now test your psychological intelligence. Profile. p.&nbsp;11. ISBN&nbsp;978-1-78283-023-8 &#8211; via Google Books. 978-1-78283-023-8

  90. DeYoung, Colin G.; Hirsh, Jacob B.; Shane, Matthew S.; Papademetris, Xenophon; Rajeevan, Nallakkandi; Gray, Jeremy R. (2010). &quot;Testing Predictions From Personality Neuroscience: Brain Structure and the Big Five&quot;. Psychological Science. 21 (6): 820&#8211;828. doi:10.1177/0956797610370159. ISSN&nbsp;0956-7976. JSTOR&nbsp;41062296. PMC&nbsp;3049165. PMID&nbsp;20435951. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3049165

  91. The 50-item IPIP representation of the Goldberg (1992) markers for the Big-Five structure at ipip.ori.org. https://ipip.ori.org/newBigFive5broadKey.htm

  92. Toegel G, Barsoux JL (2012). &quot;How to become a better leader&quot;. MIT Sloan Management Review. 53 (3): 51&#8211;60. http://sloanreview.mit.edu/article/how-to-become-a-better-leader

  93. Costa PT, McCrae RR (1992). Neo PI-R professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.[page&nbsp;needed][ISBN&nbsp;missing] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  94. The 50-item IPIP representation of the Goldberg (1992) markers for the Big-Five structure at ipip.ori.org. https://ipip.ori.org/newBigFive5broadKey.htm

  95. Laney MO (2002). The Introvert Advantage. Canada: Thomas Allen &amp; Son Limited. pp.&nbsp;28, 35. ISBN&nbsp;978-0-7611-2369-9. 978-0-7611-2369-9

  96. Friedman H, Schustack M (2016). Personality: Classic Theories and Modern Research (Sixth&nbsp;ed.). Pearson Education Inc. ISBN&nbsp;978-0-205-99793-0.[page&nbsp;needed] 978-0-205-99793-0

  97. Rothmann S, Coetzer EP (24 October 2003). &quot;The big five personality dimensions and job performance&quot;. SA Journal of Industrial Psychology. 29. doi:10.4102/sajip.v29i1.88. https://doi.org/10.4102%2Fsajip.v29i1.88

  98. Friedman H, Schustack M (2016). Personality: Classic Theories and Modern Research (Sixth&nbsp;ed.). Pearson Education Inc. ISBN&nbsp;978-0-205-99793-0.[page&nbsp;needed] 978-0-205-99793-0

  99. The 50-item IPIP representation of the Goldberg (1992) markers for the Big-Five structure at ipip.ori.org. https://ipip.ori.org/newBigFive5broadKey.htm

  100. Rothmann S, Coetzer EP (24 October 2003). &quot;The big five personality dimensions and job performance&quot;. SA Journal of Industrial Psychology. 29. doi:10.4102/sajip.v29i1.88. https://doi.org/10.4102%2Fsajip.v29i1.88

  101. Bai, Qiyu; Bai, Shiguo; Dan, Qi; Lei, Li; Wang, Pengcheng (2020-03-01). &quot;Mother phubbing and adolescent academic burnout: The mediating role of mental health and the moderating role of agreeableness and neuroticism: Personality and Individual Differences&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 155. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2019.109622. ISSN&nbsp;0191-8869. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  102. Bartneck C, Van der Hoek M, Mubin O, Al Mahmud A (March 2007). &quot;&quot;Daisy, daisy, give me your answer do!&quot; switching off a robot&quot;. Eindhoven, Netherlands: Dept. of Ind. Design, Eindhoven Univ. of Technol. pp.&nbsp;217&#8211;22. Retrieved 6 February 2013. https://ieeexplore.ieee.org/document/6251692

  103. Toegel G, Barsoux JL (2012). &quot;How to become a better leader&quot;. MIT Sloan Management Review. 53 (3): 51&#8211;60. http://sloanreview.mit.edu/article/how-to-become-a-better-leader

  104. Judge TA, Bono JE (October 2000). &quot;Five-factor model of personality and transformational leadership&quot;. The Journal of Applied Psychology. 85 (5): 751&#8211;65. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.85.5.751. PMID&nbsp;11055147. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-applied-psychology_2000-10_85_5/page/751

  105. Lim BC, Ployhart RE (August 2004). &quot;Transformational leadership: relations to the five-factor model and team performance in typical and maximum contexts&quot;. The Journal of Applied Psychology. 89 (4): 610&#8211;21. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.89.4.610. PMID&nbsp;15327348. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-applied-psychology_2004-08_89_4/page/610

  106. Sackett PR, Walmsley PT (2014). &quot;Which Personality Attributes Are Most Important in the Workplace?&quot;. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 9 (5): 538&#8211;51. doi:10.1177/1745691614543972. PMID&nbsp;26186756. S2CID&nbsp;21245818. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  107. The 50-item IPIP representation of the Goldberg (1992) markers for the Big-Five structure at ipip.ori.org. https://ipip.ori.org/newBigFive5broadKey.htm

  108. Jeronimus BF, Riese H, Sanderman R, Ormel J (October 2014). &quot;Mutual reinforcement between neuroticism and life experiences: a five-wave, 16-year study to test reciprocal causation&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 107 (4): 751&#8211;64. doi:10.1037/a0037009. PMID&nbsp;25111305. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2014-10_107_4/page/751

  109. Norris CJ, Larsen JT, Cacioppo JT (September 2007). &quot;Neuroticism is associated with larger and more prolonged electrodermal responses to emotionally evocative pictures&quot; (PDF). Psychophysiology. 44 (5): 823&#8211;26. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00551.x. PMID&nbsp;17596178. http://apsychoserver.psych.arizona.edu/jjbareprints/psyc501a/readings/Norris%20Larsen%20Cacioppo%202007%20Psychophysiology%20(SC%20Neuroticism).pdf

  110. Kagan J, Snidman N (2009). The Long Shadow of Temperament. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.[ISBN&nbsp;missing][page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  111. Fiske ST, Gilbert DT, Lindzey G (2009). Handbook of Social Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.[ISBN&nbsp;missing][page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  112. Norris CJ, Larsen JT, Cacioppo JT (September 2007). &quot;Neuroticism is associated with larger and more prolonged electrodermal responses to emotionally evocative pictures&quot; (PDF). Psychophysiology. 44 (5): 823&#8211;26. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00551.x. PMID&nbsp;17596178. http://apsychoserver.psych.arizona.edu/jjbareprints/psyc501a/readings/Norris%20Larsen%20Cacioppo%202007%20Psychophysiology%20(SC%20Neuroticism).pdf

  113. Reynaud E, El Khoury-Malhame M, Rossier J, Blin O, Khalfa S (2012). &quot;Neuroticism modifies psycho physiological responses to fearful films&quot;. PLOS ONE. 7 (3): e32413. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...732413R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0032413. PMC&nbsp;3316522. PMID&nbsp;22479326. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3316522

  114. Jeronimus BF, Riese H, Sanderman R, Ormel J (October 2014). &quot;Mutual reinforcement between neuroticism and life experiences: a five-wave, 16-year study to test reciprocal causation&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 107 (4): 751&#8211;64. doi:10.1037/a0037009. PMID&nbsp;25111305. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2014-10_107_4/page/751

  115. Jeronimus BF, Ormel J, Aleman A, Penninx BW, Riese H (November 2013). &quot;Negative and positive life events are associated with small but lasting change in neuroticism&quot;. Psychological Medicine. 43 (11): 2403&#8211;15. doi:10.1017/s0033291713000159. PMID&nbsp;23410535. S2CID&nbsp;43717734. https://archive.org/details/sim_psychological-medicine_2013-11_43_11/page/2403

  116. Jeronimus BF, Riese H, Sanderman R, Ormel J (October 2014). &quot;Mutual reinforcement between neuroticism and life experiences: a five-wave, 16-year study to test reciprocal causation&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 107 (4): 751&#8211;64. doi:10.1037/a0037009. PMID&nbsp;25111305. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2014-10_107_4/page/751

  117. Jeronimus BF, Ormel J, Aleman A, Penninx BW, Riese H (November 2013). &quot;Negative and positive life events are associated with small but lasting change in neuroticism&quot;. Psychological Medicine. 43 (11): 2403&#8211;15. doi:10.1017/s0033291713000159. PMID&nbsp;23410535. S2CID&nbsp;43717734. https://archive.org/details/sim_psychological-medicine_2013-11_43_11/page/2403

  118. Dwan T, Ownsworth T (2019). &quot;The Big Five personality factors and psychological well-being following stroke: a systematic review&quot;. Disability and Rehabilitation. 41 (10): 1119&#8211;30. doi:10.1080/09638288.2017.1419382. PMID&nbsp;29272953. S2CID&nbsp;7300458. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  119. Dolan SL (2006). Stress, Self-Esteem, Health and Work. p.&nbsp;76.[ISBN&nbsp;missing] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  120. The 50-item IPIP representation of the Goldberg (1992) markers for the Big-Five structure at ipip.ori.org. https://ipip.ori.org/newBigFive5broadKey.htm

  121. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE (2000). &quot;Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 122&#8211;35. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122. PMID&nbsp;10653510. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2000-01_78_1/page/122

  122. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  123. McCrae RR, Costa PT, Ostendorf F, Angleitner A, Hreb&#237;ckov&#225; M, Avia MD, Sanz J, S&#225;nchez-Bernardos ML, Kusdil ME, Woodfield R, Saunders PR, Smith PB (January 2000). &quot;Nature over nurture: temperament, personality, and life span development&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 173&#8211;86. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.173. PMID&nbsp;10653513. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2000-01_78_1/page/173

  124. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE (2000). &quot;Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 122&#8211;35. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122. PMID&nbsp;10653510. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2000-01_78_1/page/122

  125. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  126. Markey PM, Markey CN, Tinsley BJ (April 2004). &quot;Children's behavioral manifestations of the five-factor model of personality&quot;. Personality &amp; Social Psychology Bulletin. 30 (4): 423&#8211;32. doi:10.1177/0146167203261886. PMID&nbsp;15070472. S2CID&nbsp;33684001. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-social-psychology-bulletin_2004-04_30_4/page/423

  127. Rusalov VM (1989). &quot;Motor and communicative aspects of human temperament: a new questionnaire of the structure of temperament&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 10 (8): 817&#8211;27. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(89)90017-2. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-individual-differences_1989_10_8/page/817

  128. Strelau J (1998). Temperament: A Psychological Perspective. New York: Plenum.

  129. Rusalov VM, Trofimova IN (2007). Structure of Temperament and Its Measurement. Toronto, Canada: Psychological Services Press.[page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  130. Trofimova IN (2016). &quot;The interlocking between functional aspects of activities and a neurochemical model of temperament&quot;. In Arnold, MC (eds.). Temperaments: Individual Differences, Social and Environmental Influences and Impact on Quality of Life. New York: Nova Science Publishers. pp.&nbsp;77&#8211;147.

  131. Trofimova I, Robbins TW, Sulis WH, Uher J (April 2018). &quot;Taxonomies of psychological individual differences: biological perspectives on millennia-long challenges&quot;. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 373 (1744): 20170152. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0152. PMC&nbsp;5832678. PMID&nbsp;29483338. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5832678

  132. Trofimova I, et&nbsp;al. (2022). &quot;What's next for the neurobiology of temperament, personality and psychopathology?&quot;. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 45: 101143. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2022.101143. S2CID&nbsp;248817462. https://bura.brunel.ac.uk/handle/2438/26462

  133. Strelau J (1998). Temperament: A Psychological Perspective. New York: Plenum.

  134. Rusalov VM, Trofimova IN (2007). Structure of Temperament and Its Measurement. Toronto, Canada: Psychological Services Press.[page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  135. Trofimova IN (2016). &quot;The interlocking between functional aspects of activities and a neurochemical model of temperament&quot;. In Arnold, MC (eds.). Temperaments: Individual Differences, Social and Environmental Influences and Impact on Quality of Life. New York: Nova Science Publishers. pp.&nbsp;77&#8211;147.

  136. Trofimova IN (2016). &quot;The interlocking between functional aspects of activities and a neurochemical model of temperament&quot;. In Arnold, MC (eds.). Temperaments: Individual Differences, Social and Environmental Influences and Impact on Quality of Life. New York: Nova Science Publishers. pp.&nbsp;77&#8211;147.

  137. Trofimova IN (2010). &quot;An investigation into differences between the structure of temperament and the structure of personality&quot;. American Journal of Psychology. 123 (4): 467&#8211;80. doi:10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0467. PMID&nbsp;21291163. https://archive.org/details/sim_american-journal-of-psychology_winter-2010_123_4/page/467

  138. Jang KL, Livesley WJ, Vernon PA (September 1996). &quot;Heritability of the big five personality dimensions and their facets: a twin study&quot;. Journal of Personality. 64 (3): 577&#8211;91. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1996.tb00522.x. PMID&nbsp;8776880. S2CID&nbsp;35488176. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality_1996-09_64_3/page/577

  139. Bouchard TJ, McGue M (January 2003). &quot;Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences&quot;. Journal of Neurobiology. 54 (1): 4&#8211;45. doi:10.1002/neu.10160. PMID&nbsp;12486697. https://doi.org/10.1002%2Fneu.10160

  140. Weiss A, King JE, Hopkins WD (November 2007). &quot;A cross-setting study of chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) personality structure and development: zoological parks and Yerkes National Primate Research Center&quot;. American Journal of Primatology. 69 (11): 1264&#8211;77. doi:10.1002/ajp.20428. PMC&nbsp;2654334. PMID&nbsp;17397036. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2654334

  141. Gosling SD, John OP (1999). &quot;Personality Dimensions in Nonhuman Animals: A Cross-Species Review&quot; (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 8 (3): 69&#8211;75. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00017. S2CID&nbsp;145716504. Archived from the original on 2018-09-28. Retrieved 2016-12-05. https://web.archive.org/web/20180928211900/http://www.subjectpool.com/ed_teach/y5_ID/jc/animals/gosling_and_john_1999PersonalityInAnimals_curr_dir_psychol_sci.pdf

  142. Morton FB, Robinson LM, Brando S, Weiss A (2021). &quot;Personality structure in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)&quot;. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 135 (2): 219&#8211;231. doi:10.1037/com0000259. hdl:20.500.11820/1d4cef3b-b78b-46b3-b31c-2d1f4339cd9f. PMID&nbsp;33464108. S2CID&nbsp;231642036. https://hull-repository.worktribe.com/output/3649436

  143. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE (2000). &quot;Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 122&#8211;35. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122. PMID&nbsp;10653510. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2000-01_78_1/page/122

  144. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  145. Markey PM, Markey CN, Tinsley BJ (April 2004). &quot;Children's behavioral manifestations of the five-factor model of personality&quot;. Personality &amp; Social Psychology Bulletin. 30 (4): 423&#8211;32. doi:10.1177/0146167203261886. PMID&nbsp;15070472. S2CID&nbsp;33684001. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-social-psychology-bulletin_2004-04_30_4/page/423

  146. Soto CJ, John OP, Gosling SD, Potter J (February 2011). &quot;Age differences in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big Five domains and facets in a large cross-sectional sample&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 100 (2): 330&#8211;48. doi:10.1037/a0021717. PMID&nbsp;21171787. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2011-02_100_2/page/330

  147. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE (2000). &quot;Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 122&#8211;35. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122. PMID&nbsp;10653510. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2000-01_78_1/page/122

  148. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  149. Markey PM, Markey CN, Tinsley BJ (April 2004). &quot;Children's behavioral manifestations of the five-factor model of personality&quot;. Personality &amp; Social Psychology Bulletin. 30 (4): 423&#8211;32. doi:10.1177/0146167203261886. PMID&nbsp;15070472. S2CID&nbsp;33684001. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-social-psychology-bulletin_2004-04_30_4/page/423

  150. Soto CJ (August 2016). &quot;The Little Six Personality Dimensions From Early Childhood to Early Adulthood: Mean-Level Age and Gender Differences in Parents' Reports&quot;. Journal of Personality. 84 (4): 409&#8211;22. doi:10.1111/jopy.12168. PMID&nbsp;25728032. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  151. Soto CJ (August 2016). &quot;The Little Six Personality Dimensions From Early Childhood to Early Adulthood: Mean-Level Age and Gender Differences in Parents' Reports&quot;. Journal of Personality. 84 (4): 409&#8211;22. doi:10.1111/jopy.12168. PMID&nbsp;25728032. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  152. Soto CJ (August 2016). &quot;The Little Six Personality Dimensions From Early Childhood to Early Adulthood: Mean-Level Age and Gender Differences in Parents' Reports&quot;. Journal of Personality. 84 (4): 409&#8211;22. doi:10.1111/jopy.12168. PMID&nbsp;25728032. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  153. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE (2000). &quot;Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 122&#8211;35. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122. PMID&nbsp;10653510. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2000-01_78_1/page/122

  154. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  155. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  156. Soto CJ (August 2016). &quot;The Little Six Personality Dimensions From Early Childhood to Early Adulthood: Mean-Level Age and Gender Differences in Parents' Reports&quot;. Journal of Personality. 84 (4): 409&#8211;22. doi:10.1111/jopy.12168. PMID&nbsp;25728032. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  157. Lewis M (2001). &quot;Issues in the study of personality development&quot;. Psychological Inquiry. 12 (2): 67&#8211;83. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1202_02. S2CID&nbsp;144557981. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  158. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE (2000). &quot;Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 122&#8211;35. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122. PMID&nbsp;10653510. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2000-01_78_1/page/122

  159. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  160. Markey PM, Markey CN, Tinsley BJ (April 2004). &quot;Children's behavioral manifestations of the five-factor model of personality&quot;. Personality &amp; Social Psychology Bulletin. 30 (4): 423&#8211;32. doi:10.1177/0146167203261886. PMID&nbsp;15070472. S2CID&nbsp;33684001. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-social-psychology-bulletin_2004-04_30_4/page/423

  161. Goldberg LR (2001). &quot;Analyses of Digman's child- personality data: Derivation of Big Five Factor Scores from each of six samples&quot;. Journal of Personality. 69 (5): 709&#8211;43. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.695161. PMID&nbsp;11575511. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality_2001-10_69_5/page/709

  162. Mervielde I, De Fruyt F (1999). &quot;Construction of the Hierarchical Personality Inventory for Children (Hi- PIC).&quot;. In Mervielde ID, De Fruyt F, Ostendorf F (eds.). Personality psychology in Europe: Proceedings of the Eighth European Conference on Personality. Tilburg University Press. pp.&nbsp;107&#8211;27.

  163. Resing WC, Bleichrodt N, Dekker PH (1999). &quot;Measuring personality traits in the classroom&quot; (PDF). European Journal of Personality. 13 (6): 493&#8211;509. doi:10.1002/(sici)1099-0984(199911/12)13:6&lt;493::aid-per355&gt;3.0.co;2-v. hdl:1871/18675. S2CID&nbsp;56322465. https://research.vu.nl/ws/files/1318382/resing%20European%20Journal%20of%20Personality,%2013(12).pdf

  164. Markey PM, Markey CN, Ericksen AJ, Tinsley BJ (2002). &quot;A preliminary validation of preadolescents' self-reports using the Five-Factor Model of personality&quot;. Journal of Research in Personality. 36 (2): 173&#8211;81. doi:10.1006/jrpe.2001.2341. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  165. Scholte RH, van Aken MA, van Lieshout CF (December 1997). &quot;Adolescent personality factors in self-ratings and peer nominations and their prediction of peer acceptance and peer rejection&quot;. Journal of Personality Assessment. 69 (3): 534&#8211;54. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6903_8. PMID&nbsp;9501483. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-assessment_1997-12_69_3/page/534

  166. Halverson CF, Kohnstamm GA, Martin RP, eds. (1994). The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. https://archive.org/details/developingstruct0000unse

  167. Markey PM, Markey CN, Tinsley BJ (April 2004). &quot;Children's behavioral manifestations of the five-factor model of personality&quot;. Personality &amp; Social Psychology Bulletin. 30 (4): 423&#8211;32. doi:10.1177/0146167203261886. PMID&nbsp;15070472. S2CID&nbsp;33684001. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-social-psychology-bulletin_2004-04_30_4/page/423

  168. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  169. Markey PM, Markey CN, Tinsley BJ (April 2004). &quot;Children's behavioral manifestations of the five-factor model of personality&quot;. Personality &amp; Social Psychology Bulletin. 30 (4): 423&#8211;32. doi:10.1177/0146167203261886. PMID&nbsp;15070472. S2CID&nbsp;33684001. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-social-psychology-bulletin_2004-04_30_4/page/423

  170. Halverson CF, Kohnstamm GA, Martin RP, eds. (1994). The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. https://archive.org/details/developingstruct0000unse

  171. Kohnstamm GA, Halverson Jr CF, Mervielde I, Havill VL, eds. (1998). Parental descriptions of child personality: Developmental antecedents of the Big Five?. Psychology Press.[ISBN&nbsp;missing][page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  172. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  173. Markey PM, Markey CN, Tinsley BJ (April 2004). &quot;Children's behavioral manifestations of the five-factor model of personality&quot;. Personality &amp; Social Psychology Bulletin. 30 (4): 423&#8211;32. doi:10.1177/0146167203261886. PMID&nbsp;15070472. S2CID&nbsp;33684001. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-social-psychology-bulletin_2004-04_30_4/page/423

  174. Halverson CF, Kohnstamm GA, Martin RP, eds. (1994). The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. https://archive.org/details/developingstruct0000unse

  175. Kohnstamm GA, Halverson Jr CF, Mervielde I, Havill VL, eds. (1998). Parental descriptions of child personality: Developmental antecedents of the Big Five?. Psychology Press.[ISBN&nbsp;missing][page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  176. Mervielde I, De Fruyt F, Jarmuz S (May 1998). &quot;Linking openness and intellect in childhood and adulthood.&quot;. In Kohnstamm GA, Halverson CF, Mervielde I, Havill VL (eds.). Parental descriptions of child personality: Developmental antecedents of the Big Five. Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum. pp.&nbsp;105&#8211;26. ISBN&nbsp;978-0-8058-2301-1. 978-0-8058-2301-1

  177. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  178. Markey PM, Markey CN, Tinsley BJ (April 2004). &quot;Children's behavioral manifestations of the five-factor model of personality&quot;. Personality &amp; Social Psychology Bulletin. 30 (4): 423&#8211;32. doi:10.1177/0146167203261886. PMID&nbsp;15070472. S2CID&nbsp;33684001. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-social-psychology-bulletin_2004-04_30_4/page/423

  179. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  180. Markey PM, Markey CN, Tinsley BJ (April 2004). &quot;Children's behavioral manifestations of the five-factor model of personality&quot;. Personality &amp; Social Psychology Bulletin. 30 (4): 423&#8211;32. doi:10.1177/0146167203261886. PMID&nbsp;15070472. S2CID&nbsp;33684001. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-social-psychology-bulletin_2004-04_30_4/page/423

  181. John OP, Srivastava S (1999). &quot;The Big-Five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and theoretical perspectives&quot; (PDF). In Pervin LA, John OP (eds.). Handbook of personality: Theory and research. Vol.&nbsp;2. New York: Guilford Press. pp.&nbsp;102&#8211;38. http://pages.uoregon.edu/sanjay/pubs/bigfive.pdf

  182. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  183. Markey PM, Markey CN, Tinsley BJ (April 2004). &quot;Children's behavioral manifestations of the five-factor model of personality&quot;. Personality &amp; Social Psychology Bulletin. 30 (4): 423&#8211;32. doi:10.1177/0146167203261886. PMID&nbsp;15070472. S2CID&nbsp;33684001. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-social-psychology-bulletin_2004-04_30_4/page/423

  184. Halverson CF, Kohnstamm GA, Martin RP, eds. (1994). The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. https://archive.org/details/developingstruct0000unse

  185. Kohnstamm GA, Halverson Jr CF, Mervielde I, Havill VL, eds. (1998). Parental descriptions of child personality: Developmental antecedents of the Big Five?. Psychology Press.[ISBN&nbsp;missing][page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  186. Soto C, Tackett J (2015). &quot;Personality Traits in Childhood and Adolescence: Structure, Development, and Outcomes&quot; (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 24: 358&#8211;62. doi:10.1177/0963721415589345. S2CID&nbsp;29475747. http://www.colby.edu/psych/wp-content/uploads/sites/50/2013/08/Soto_Tackett_2015.pdf

  187. Soto CJ (August 2016). &quot;The Little Six Personality Dimensions From Early Childhood to Early Adulthood: Mean-Level Age and Gender Differences in Parents' Reports&quot;. Journal of Personality. 84 (4): 409&#8211;22. doi:10.1111/jopy.12168. PMID&nbsp;25728032. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  188. Soto C, Tackett J (2015). &quot;Personality Traits in Childhood and Adolescence: Structure, Development, and Outcomes&quot; (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 24: 358&#8211;62. doi:10.1177/0963721415589345. S2CID&nbsp;29475747. http://www.colby.edu/psych/wp-content/uploads/sites/50/2013/08/Soto_Tackett_2015.pdf

  189. Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W (January 2006). &quot;Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies&quot;. Psychological Bulletin. 132 (1): 1&#8211;25. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1. PMID&nbsp;16435954. S2CID&nbsp;16606495. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  190. McCrae RR, Costa PT, Ostendorf F, Angleitner A, Hreb&#237;ckov&#225; M, Avia MD, Sanz J, S&#225;nchez-Bernardos ML, Kusdil ME, Woodfield R, Saunders PR, Smith PB (January 2000). &quot;Nature over nurture: temperament, personality, and life span development&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 173&#8211;86. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.173. PMID&nbsp;10653513. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2000-01_78_1/page/173

  191. Soto C, Tackett J (2015). &quot;Personality Traits in Childhood and Adolescence: Structure, Development, and Outcomes&quot; (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 24: 358&#8211;62. doi:10.1177/0963721415589345. S2CID&nbsp;29475747. http://www.colby.edu/psych/wp-content/uploads/sites/50/2013/08/Soto_Tackett_2015.pdf

  192. Roberts BW, DelVecchio WF (January 2000). &quot;The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies&quot; (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 126 (1): 3&#8211;25. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3. PMID&nbsp;10668348. S2CID&nbsp;7484026. http://jenni.uchicago.edu/Spencer_Conference/Representative%20Papers/Roberts%20&%20DelVecchio,%202000.pdf

  193. Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W (January 2006). &quot;Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies&quot;. Psychological Bulletin. 132 (1): 1&#8211;25. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1. PMID&nbsp;16435954. S2CID&nbsp;16606495. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  194. Soto C, Tackett J (2015). &quot;Personality Traits in Childhood and Adolescence: Structure, Development, and Outcomes&quot; (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 24: 358&#8211;62. doi:10.1177/0963721415589345. S2CID&nbsp;29475747. http://www.colby.edu/psych/wp-content/uploads/sites/50/2013/08/Soto_Tackett_2015.pdf

  195. Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W (January 2006). &quot;Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies&quot;. Psychological Bulletin. 132 (1): 1&#8211;25. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1. PMID&nbsp;16435954. S2CID&nbsp;16606495. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  196. Soto C, Tackett J (2015). &quot;Personality Traits in Childhood and Adolescence: Structure, Development, and Outcomes&quot; (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 24: 358&#8211;62. doi:10.1177/0963721415589345. S2CID&nbsp;29475747. http://www.colby.edu/psych/wp-content/uploads/sites/50/2013/08/Soto_Tackett_2015.pdf

  197. Soto CJ (August 2016). &quot;The Little Six Personality Dimensions From Early Childhood to Early Adulthood: Mean-Level Age and Gender Differences in Parents' Reports&quot;. Journal of Personality. 84 (4): 409&#8211;22. doi:10.1111/jopy.12168. PMID&nbsp;25728032. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  198. Soto C, Tackett J (2015). &quot;Personality Traits in Childhood and Adolescence: Structure, Development, and Outcomes&quot; (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 24: 358&#8211;62. doi:10.1177/0963721415589345. S2CID&nbsp;29475747. http://www.colby.edu/psych/wp-content/uploads/sites/50/2013/08/Soto_Tackett_2015.pdf

  199. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE (2000). &quot;Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 122&#8211;35. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122. PMID&nbsp;10653510. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2000-01_78_1/page/122

  200. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  201. Lemery KS, Goldsmith HH, Klinnert MD, Mrazek DA (January 1999). &quot;Developmental models of infant and childhood temperament&quot;. Developmental Psychology. 35 (1): 189&#8211;204. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.189. PMID&nbsp;9923474. https://archive.org/details/sim_developmental-psychology_1999-01_35_1/page/189

  202. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE (2000). &quot;Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 122&#8211;35. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122. PMID&nbsp;10653510. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2000-01_78_1/page/122

  203. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  204. Buss A, Plomin R (1984). Temperament: early developing personality trait. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

  205. Kagan J, Snidman N (2009). The Long Shadow of Temperament. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.[ISBN&nbsp;missing][page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  206. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  207. Goldberg LR (2001). &quot;Analyses of Digman's child- personality data: Derivation of Big Five Factor Scores from each of six samples&quot;. Journal of Personality. 69 (5): 709&#8211;43. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.695161. PMID&nbsp;11575511. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality_2001-10_69_5/page/709

  208. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey KL, Fisher P (2001). &quot;Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: the Children's Behavior Questionnaire&quot;. Child Development. 72 (5): 1394&#8211;408. CiteSeerX&nbsp;10.1.1.398.8830. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00355. PMID&nbsp;11699677. /wiki/CiteSeerX_(identifier)

  209. John OP, Caspi A, Robins RW, Moffitt TE, Stouthamer-Loeber M (February 1994). &quot;The &quot;little five&quot;: exploring the nomological network of the five-factor model of personality in adolescent boys&quot;. Child Development. 65 (1): 160&#8211;78. doi:10.2307/1131373. JSTOR&nbsp;1131373. PMID&nbsp;8131645. https://archive.org/details/sim_child-development_1994-02_65_1/page/160

  210. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  211. Eaton WO (1994). &quot;Temperament, development, and the Five-Factor Model: Lessons from activity level&quot;. In Halverson CF, Kohnstamm GA, Martin RP (eds.). The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. pp.&nbsp;173&#8211;87.

  212. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  213. Hawley PH (1999). &quot;The ontogenesis of social dominance: A strategy-based evolutionary perspective&quot;. Developmental Review. 19: 97&#8211;132. CiteSeerX&nbsp;10.1.1.459.4755. doi:10.1006/drev.1998.0470. /wiki/CiteSeerX_(identifier)

  214. Hawley PH, Little TD (1999). &quot;On winning some and losing some: A social relations approach to social dominance in toddlers&quot;. Merrill Palmer Quarterly. 45: 185&#8211;214. https://archive.org/details/sim_merrill-palmer-quarterly_1999-01_45_1/page/n188

  215. Sherif M, Harvey O, White BJ, Hood WR, Sherif C (1961). Intergroup conflict and cooperation: The robbers' cave experiment. Norman, OK: University of Oklahoma Press. OCLC&nbsp;953442127. https://www.free-ebooks.net/ebook/Intergroup-Conflict-and-Cooperation-The-Robbers-Cave-Experiment/pdf?dl&preview

  216. Keating CF, Heltman KR (1994). &quot;Dominance and deception in children and adults: Are leaders the best misleaders?&quot;. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 20 (3): 312&#8211;21. doi:10.1177/0146167294203009. S2CID&nbsp;19252480. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-social-psychology-bulletin_1994-06_20_3/page/312

  217. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  218. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  219. Asendorpf JB (1990). &quot;Development of inhibition during childhood: Evidence for situational specificity and a two-factor model&quot;. Developmental Psychology. 26 (5): 721&#8211;30. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.26.5.721. https://archive.org/details/sim_developmental-psychology_1990-09_26_5/page/721

  220. Kagan J, Snidman N (2009). The Long Shadow of Temperament. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.[ISBN&nbsp;missing][page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  221. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  222. Asendorpf JB, Meier GH (1993). &quot;Personality effects on children's speech in everyday life: Sociability-mediated exposure and shyness-mediated re-activity to social situations&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 64 (6): 1072&#8211;83. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.6.1072. PMID&nbsp;8326470. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_1993-06_64_6/page/1072

  223. Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). &quot;Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2&#8211;32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID&nbsp;12553411. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  224. Harrist AW, Zaia AF, Bates JE, Dodge KA, Pettit GS (April 1997). &quot;Subtypes of social withdrawal in early childhood: sociometric status and social-cognitive differences across four years&quot;. Child Development. 68 (2): 278&#8211;94. doi:10.2307/1131850. JSTOR&nbsp;1131850. PMID&nbsp;9180002. https://archive.org/details/sim_child-development_1997-04_68_2/page/278

  225. Mathiesen KS, Tambs K (March 1999). &quot;The EAS temperament questionnaire &#8211; factor structure, age trends, reliability, and stability in a Norwegian sample&quot;. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 40 (3): 431&#8211;39. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00460. PMID&nbsp;10190344. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  226. McCrae RR, Costa PT (1990). Personality in adulthood. New York: The Guildford Press.[page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  227. Kagan J, Snidman N (2009). The Long Shadow of Temperament. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.[ISBN&nbsp;missing][page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  228. Cobb-Clark DA, Schurer S (2012). &quot;The stability of big-five personality traits&quot; (PDF). Economics Letters. 115 (2): 11&#8211;15. doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2011.11.015. S2CID&nbsp;12086995. http://melbourneinstitute.unimelb.edu.au/downloads/working_paper_series/wp2011n21.pdf

  229. Srivastava S, John OP, Gosling SD, Potter J (May 2003). &quot;Development of personality in early and middle adulthood: set like plaster or persistent change?&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 84 (5): 1041&#8211;53. CiteSeerX&nbsp;10.1.1.499.4124. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1041. PMID&nbsp;12757147. S2CID&nbsp;14790757. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2003-05_84_5/page/1041

  230. Soto CJ, John OP, Gosling SD, Potter J (February 2011). &quot;Age differences in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big Five domains and facets in a large cross-sectional sample&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 100 (2): 330&#8211;48. doi:10.1037/a0021717. PMID&nbsp;21171787. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2011-02_100_2/page/330

  231. Roberts BW, Mroczek D (February 2008). &quot;Personality Trait Change in Adulthood&quot;. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 17 (1): 31&#8211;35. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x. PMC&nbsp;2743415. PMID&nbsp;19756219. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2743415

  232. Fleeson W (2001). &quot;Towards a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 80 (6): 1011&#8211;27. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.1011. PMID&nbsp;11414368. S2CID&nbsp;13805210. /wiki/William_Fleeson

  233. M&#245;ttus R, Johnson W, Starr JM, Dearya IJ (June 2012). &quot;Correlates of personality trait levels and their changes in very old age: The Lothian Birth Cohort 1921&quot; (PDF). Journal of Research in Personality. 46 (3): 271&#8211;78. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2012.02.004. hdl:20.500.11820/b6b6961d-902f-48e0-bf25-f505a659a056. S2CID&nbsp;53117809. https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/15012527/Figure_1_without_legend.pdf

  234. Cavallera G, Passerini A, Pepe A (2013). &quot;Personality and gender in swimmers in indoor practice at leisure level&quot;. Social Behavior and Personality. 41 (4): 693&#8211;704. doi:10.2224/sbp.2013.41.4.693. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  235. Falk, Armin; Hermle, Johannes (2018-10-19). &quot;Relationship of gender differences in preferences to economic development and gender equality&quot;. Science. 362 (6412): eaas9899. doi:10.1126/science.aas9899. hdl:10419/193353. ISSN&nbsp;0036-8075. PMID&nbsp;30337384. https://doi.org/10.1126%2Fscience.aas9899

  236. Costa PT, Terracciano A, McCrae RR (August 2001). &quot;Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: robust and surprising findings&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (2): 322&#8211;31. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.322. PMID&nbsp;11519935. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2001-08_81_2/page/322

  237. Schmitt DP, Realo A, Voracek M, Allik J (January 2008). &quot;Why can't a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in Big Five personality traits across 55 cultures&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 94 (1): 168&#8211;82. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.168. PMID&nbsp;18179326. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2008-01_94_1/page/168

  238. Costa PT, Terracciano A, McCrae RR (August 2001). &quot;Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: robust and surprising findings&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (2): 322&#8211;31. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.322. PMID&nbsp;11519935. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2001-08_81_2/page/322

  239. Schmitt DP, Realo A, Voracek M, Allik J (January 2008). &quot;Why can't a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in Big Five personality traits across 55 cultures&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 94 (1): 168&#8211;82. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.168. PMID&nbsp;18179326. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2008-01_94_1/page/168

  240. Harris, J. R. (2006). No two alike: Human nature and human individuality. WW Norton &amp; Company.

  241. Jefferson T, Herbst JH, McCrae RR (1998). &quot;Associations between birth order and personality traits: Evidence from self-reports and observer ratings&quot;. Journal of Research in Personality. 32 (4): 498&#8211;509. doi:10.1006/jrpe.1998.2233. https://zenodo.org/record/1229908

  242. Damian RI, Roberts BW (October 2015). &quot;The associations of birth order with personality and intelligence in a representative sample of U.S. high school students&quot;. Journal of Research in Personality. 58: 96&#8211;105. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2015.05.005. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  243. Thompson RL, Brossart DF, Carlozzi AF, Miville ML (September 2002). &quot;Five-factor model (Big Five) personality traits and universal-diverse orientation in counselor trainees&quot;. The Journal of Psychology. 136 (5): 561&#8211;72. doi:10.1080/00223980209605551. PMID&nbsp;12431039. S2CID&nbsp;22076221. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-psychology_2002-09_136_5/page/561

  244. Friedman H, Schustack M (2016). Personality: Classic Theories and Modern Research (Sixth&nbsp;ed.). Pearson Education Inc. ISBN&nbsp;978-0-205-99793-0.[page&nbsp;needed] 978-0-205-99793-0

  245. Ostendorf, F. (1990). Sprache und Persoenlichkeitsstruktur: Zur Validitaet des Funf-Factoren-Modells der Persoenlichkeit. Regensburg, Germany: S. Roderer Verlag.[page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  246. Trull TJ, Geary DC (October 1997). &quot;Comparison of the big-five factor structure across samples of Chinese and American adults&quot;. Journal of Personality Assessment. 69 (2): 324&#8211;41. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6902_6. PMID&nbsp;9392894. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-assessment_1997-10_69_2/page/324

  247. Lodhi PH, Deo S, Belhekar VM (2002). &quot;The Five-Factor model of personality in Indian context: measurement and correlates.&quot;. In McCrae RR, Allik J (eds.). The Five-Factor model of personality across cultures. New York: Kluwer Academic Publisher. pp.&nbsp;227&#8211;48.

  248. McCrae RR (2002). &quot;NEO-PI-R data from 36 cultures: Further Intercultural comparisons.&quot;. In McCrae RR, Allik J (eds.). The Five-Factor model of personality across cultures. New York: Kluwer Academic Publisher. pp.&nbsp;105&#8211;25.

  249. Thompson ER (2008). &quot;Development and validation of an international English big-five mini-markers&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 45 (6): 542&#8211;48. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.06.013. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-individual-differences_2008-10_45_6/page/542

  250. Cheung FM, van de Vijver FJ, Leong FT (October 2011). &quot;Toward a new approach to the study of personality in culture&quot;. The American Psychologist. 66 (7): 593&#8211;603. doi:10.1037/a0022389. PMID&nbsp;21261408. S2CID&nbsp;615860. Archived from the original on 2013-05-18. Retrieved 2013-01-16. https://web.archive.org/web/20130518090148/http://arno.uvt.nl/show.cgi?fid=120612

  251. Eap, Sopagna; DeGarmo, David S.; Kawakami, Ayaka; Hara, Shelley N.; Hall, Gordon C.N.; Teten, Andra L. (September 2008). &quot;Culture and Personality Among European American and Asian American Men&quot;. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology. 39 (5): 630&#8211;643. doi:10.1177/0022022108321310. ISSN&nbsp;0022-0221. PMC&nbsp;2630227. PMID&nbsp;19169434. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2630227

  252. Benet-Mart&#237;nez, Ver&#243;nica; Karakitapoglu-Ayg&#252;n, Zahide (January 2003). &quot;The Interplay Of Cultural Syndromes And Personality In Predicting Life Satisfaction: Comparing Asian Americans and European Americans&quot;. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 34 (1): 38&#8211;60. doi:10.1177/0022022102239154. ISSN&nbsp;0022-0221. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0022022102239154

  253. McCrae RR, Terracciano A (September 2005). &quot;Personality profiles of cultures: aggregate personality traits&quot; (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 89 (3): 407&#8211;25. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.407. PMID&nbsp;16248722. http://idiprints.knjiznica.idi.hr/649/1/JoPaSP%202005_3%20Mccrae%20et%20al..pdf

  254. Hofstede, Geert; Bond, Michael H. (1984). &quot;Hofstede's Culture Dimensions: An Independent Validation Using Rokeach's Value Survey&quot;. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 15 (4): 417&#8211;433. doi:10.1177/0022002184015004003. ISSN&nbsp;0022-0221. S2CID&nbsp;145651845. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/0022002184015004003

  255. Mooradian, Todd A.; Swan, K. Scott (2006-06-01). &quot;Personality-and-culture: The case of national extraversion and word-of-mouth&quot;. Journal of Business Research. Special Section - The 2005 La Londe Seminar. 59 (6): 778&#8211;785. doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.01.015. ISSN&nbsp;0148-2963. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0148296306000312

  256. Barcel&#243; J (2017). &quot;National Personality Traits and Regime Type: A Cross-National Study of 47 Countries&quot;. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 48 (2): 195&#8211;216. doi:10.1177/0022022116678324. S2CID&nbsp;151607260. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  257. Szirmak Z, De Raad B (1994). &quot;Taxonomy and structure of Hungarian personality traits&quot;. European Journal of Personality. 8 (2): 95&#8211;117. doi:10.1002/per.2410080203. S2CID&nbsp;145275826. https://archive.org/details/sim_european-journal-of-personality_1994-06_8_2/page/95

  258. De Fruyt F, McCrae RR, Szirm&#225;k Z, Nagy J (September 2004). &quot;The Five-factor Personality Inventory as a measure of the Five-factor Model: Belgian, American, and Hungarian comparisons with the NEO-PI-R&quot;. Assessment. 11 (3): 207&#8211;15. doi:10.1177/1073191104265800. PMID&nbsp;15358876. S2CID&nbsp;29733250. https://archive.org/details/sim_assessment_2004-09_11_3/page/207

  259. Robins Wahlin TB, Byrne GJ (October 2011). &quot;Personality changes in Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review&quot;. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 26 (10): 1019&#8211;29. doi:10.1002/gps.2655. PMID&nbsp;21905097. S2CID&nbsp;40949990. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  260. Hunt A, Martyr A, Gamble LD, Morris RG, Thom JM, Pentecost C, Clare L (June 2023). &quot;The associations between personality traits and quality of life, satisfaction with life, and well-being over time in people with dementia and their caregivers: findings from the IDEAL programme&quot;. BMC Geriatrics. 23 (1): 354. doi:10.1186/s12877-023-04075-x. PMC&nbsp;10242791. PMID&nbsp;37280511. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10242791

  261. Widiger TA, Costa PT. Jr. &quot;Five-Factor model personality disorder research&quot;. In: Costa Paul T Jr, Widiger Thomas A., editors. Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality. 2nd. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association; 2002. pp. 59&#8211;87. 2002. [ISBN&nbsp;missing] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  262. Mullins-Sweatt SN, Widiger TA (2006). &quot;The five-factor model of personality disorder: A translation across science and practice.&quot;. In Krueger R, Tackett J (eds.). Personality and psychopathology: Building bridges. New York: Guilford. pp.&nbsp;39&#8211;70.[ISBN&nbsp;missing] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  263. Clark LA (2007). &quot;Assessment and diagnosis of personality disorder: perennial issues and an emerging reconceptualization&quot;. Annual Review of Psychology. 58: 227&#8211;57. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190200. PMID&nbsp;16903806. https://zenodo.org/record/1134186

  264. Trofimova IN (2016). &quot;The interlocking between functional aspects of activities and a neurochemical model of temperament&quot;. In Arnold, MC (eds.). Temperaments: Individual Differences, Social and Environmental Influences and Impact on Quality of Life. New York: Nova Science Publishers. pp.&nbsp;77&#8211;147.

  265. Trofimova I, Robbins TW (May 2016). &quot;Temperament and arousal systems: A new synthesis of differential psychology and functional neurochemistry&quot;. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 64: 382&#8211;402. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.008. hdl:11375/26202. PMID&nbsp;26969100. S2CID&nbsp;13937324. https://www.repository.cam.ac.uk/handle/1810/274784

  266. Trofimova IN (2016). &quot;The interlocking between functional aspects of activities and a neurochemical model of temperament&quot;. In Arnold, MC (eds.). Temperaments: Individual Differences, Social and Environmental Influences and Impact on Quality of Life. New York: Nova Science Publishers. pp.&nbsp;77&#8211;147.

  267. Trofimova I, Sulis W (2016). &quot;Benefits of Distinguishing between Physical and Social-Verbal Aspects of Behavior: An Example of Generalized Anxiety&quot;. Frontiers in Psychology. 7: 338. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00338. PMC&nbsp;4789559. PMID&nbsp;27014146. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4789559

  268. Trofimova I, Christiansen J (April 2016). &quot;Coupling of Temperament with Mental Illness in Four Age Groups&quot;. Psychological Reports. 118 (2): 387&#8211;412. doi:10.1177/0033294116639430. PMID&nbsp;27154370. S2CID&nbsp;24465522. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  269. Depue R, Fu Y (2012). &quot;Neurobiology and neurochemistry of temperament in adults&quot;. In Zentner M, Shiner R (eds.). Handbook of Temperament. New York: Guilford Publications. pp.&nbsp;368&#8211;99.

  270. Bagby RM, Sellbom M, Costa PT, Widiger TA (April 2008). &quot;PredictingDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV personality disorders with the five-factor model of personality and the personality psychopathology five&quot;. Personality and Mental Health. 2 (2): 55&#8211;69. doi:10.1002/pmh.33. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  271. &quot;The five-factor model and personality disorder empirical literature: A meta-analytic review.&quot; LM Saulsman, AC Page, Clinical Psychology Review, 2004 &#8211; Elsevier Science [ISBN&nbsp;missing][page&nbsp;needed] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  272. Kessler RC, Chiu WT, Demler O, Merikangas KR, Walters EE (June 2005). &quot;Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication&quot;. Archives of General Psychiatry. 62 (6): 617&#8211;27. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617. PMC&nbsp;2847357. PMID&nbsp;15939839. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2847357

  273. Kessler RC, Chiu WT, Demler O, Merikangas KR, Walters EE (June 2005). &quot;Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication&quot;. Archives of General Psychiatry. 62 (6): 617&#8211;27. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617. PMC&nbsp;2847357. PMID&nbsp;15939839. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2847357

  274. Compton WM, Conway KP, Stinson FS, Colliver JD, Grant BF (June 2005). &quot;Prevalence, correlates, and comorbidity of DSM-IV antisocial personality syndromes and alcohol and specific drug use disorders in the United States: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions&quot;. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 66 (6): 677&#8211;85. doi:10.4088/jcp.v66n0602. PMID&nbsp;15960559. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  275. Hasin DS, Goodwin RD, Stinson FS, Grant BF (October 2005). &quot;Epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcoholism and Related Conditions&quot;. Archives of General Psychiatry. 62 (10): 1097&#8211;106. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.10.1097. PMID&nbsp;16203955. https://doi.org/10.1001%2Farchpsyc.62.10.1097

  276. Fehrman, Elaine; Egan, Vincent; Gorban, Alexander N.; Levesley, Jeremy; Mirkes, Evgeny M.; Muhammad, Awaz K. (2019). Personality Traits and Drug Consumption. A Story Told by Data. Springer, Cham. arXiv:2001.06520. doi:10.1007/978-3-030-10442-9. ISBN&nbsp;978-3-030-10441-2. S2CID&nbsp;151160405. 978-3-030-10441-2

  277. Fehrman, Elaine; Egan, Vincent; Gorban, Alexander N.; Levesley, Jeremy; Mirkes, Evgeny M.; Muhammad, Awaz K. (2019). Personality Traits and Drug Consumption. A Story Told by Data. Springer, Cham. arXiv:2001.06520. doi:10.1007/978-3-030-10442-9. ISBN&nbsp;978-3-030-10441-2. S2CID&nbsp;151160405. 978-3-030-10441-2

  278. Khan AA, Jacobson KC, Gardner CO, Prescott CA, Kendler KS (March 2005). &quot;Personality and comorbidity of common psychiatric disorders&quot;. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 186 (3): 190&#8211;96. doi:10.1192/bjp.186.3.190. PMID&nbsp;15738498. https://doi.org/10.1192%2Fbjp.186.3.190

  279. Cuijpers P, Smit F, Penninx BW, de Graaf R, ten Have M, Beekman AT (October 2010). &quot;Economic costs of neuroticism: a population-based study&quot;. Archives of General Psychiatry. 67 (10): 1086&#8211;93. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.130. PMID&nbsp;20921124. https://doi.org/10.1001%2Farchgenpsychiatry.2010.130

  280. Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D (September 2010). &quot;Linking &quot;big&quot; personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis&quot;. Psychological Bulletin. 136 (5): 768&#8211;821. doi:10.1037/a0020327. PMID&nbsp;20804236. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/46094267

  281. Bogg T, Roberts BW (November 2004). &quot;Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: a meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality&quot;. Psychological Bulletin. 130 (6): 887&#8211;919. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.887. PMID&nbsp;15535742. https://archive.org/details/sim_psychological-bulletin_2004-11_130_6/page/887

  282. Roberts BW, Kuncel NR, Shiner R, Caspi A, Goldberg LR (December 2007). &quot;The Power of Personality: The Comparative Validity of Personality Traits, Socioeconomic Status, and Cognitive Ability for Predicting Important Life Outcomes&quot; (PDF). Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2 (4): 313&#8211;45. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00047.x. PMC&nbsp;4499872. PMID&nbsp;26151971. http://jenni.uchicago.edu/Spencer_Conference/Representative%20Papers/Roberts%20et%20al,%202007%20PPS%20power%20of%20personality.pdf

  283. Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D (September 2010). &quot;Linking &quot;big&quot; personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis&quot;. Psychological Bulletin. 136 (5): 768&#8211;821. doi:10.1037/a0020327. PMID&nbsp;20804236. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/46094267

  284. Jeronimus BF, Kotov R, Riese H, Ormel J (October 2016). &quot;Neuroticism's prospective association with mental disorders halves after adjustment for baseline symptoms and psychiatric history, but the adjusted association hardly decays with time: a meta-analysis on 59 longitudinal/prospective studies with 443 313 participants&quot;. Psychological Medicine. 46 (14): 2883&#8211;906. doi:10.1017/S0033291716001653. PMID&nbsp;27523506. S2CID&nbsp;23548727. https://zenodo.org/record/895885

  285. Jeronimus BF, Kotov R, Riese H, Ormel J (October 2016). &quot;Neuroticism's prospective association with mental disorders halves after adjustment for baseline symptoms and psychiatric history, but the adjusted association hardly decays with time: a meta-analysis on 59 longitudinal/prospective studies with 443 313 participants&quot;. Psychological Medicine. 46 (14): 2883&#8211;906. doi:10.1017/S0033291716001653. PMID&nbsp;27523506. S2CID&nbsp;23548727. https://zenodo.org/record/895885

  286. Livesley WJ (2001). Handbook of Personality Disorders. New York: The Guildford Press. pp.&nbsp;84&#8211;104. ISBN&nbsp;978-1-57230-629-5. OCLC&nbsp;783011161. 978-1-57230-629-5

  287. Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D (September 2010). &quot;Linking &quot;big&quot; personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis&quot;. Psychological Bulletin. 136 (5): 768&#8211;821. doi:10.1037/a0020327. PMID&nbsp;20804236. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/46094267

  288. Ormel J, Jeronimus BF, Kotov R, Riese H, Bos EH, Hankin B, Rosmalen JG, Oldehinkel AJ (July 2013). &quot;Neuroticism and common mental disorders: meaning and utility of a complex relationship&quot;. Clinical Psychology Review. 33 (5): 686&#8211;97. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.04.003. PMC&nbsp;4382368. PMID&nbsp;23702592. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4382368

  289. Millon T, Krueger R, Simonsen E (2011). Contemporary Directions in Psychopathology: Scientific Foundations of the DSM-IV and ICD-11. Guilford Press.

  290. Krueger R, Tackett L (2006). Personality and Psychopathology. Guilford Press.[page&nbsp;needed][ISBN&nbsp;missing] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  291. Jeronimus BF, Kotov R, Riese H, Ormel J (October 2016). &quot;Neuroticism's prospective association with mental disorders halves after adjustment for baseline symptoms and psychiatric history, but the adjusted association hardly decays with time: a meta-analysis on 59 longitudinal/prospective studies with 443 313 participants&quot;. Psychological Medicine. 46 (14): 2883&#8211;906. doi:10.1017/S0033291716001653. PMID&nbsp;27523506. S2CID&nbsp;23548727. https://zenodo.org/record/895885

  292. Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D (September 2010). &quot;Linking &quot;big&quot; personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis&quot;. Psychological Bulletin. 136 (5): 768&#8211;821. doi:10.1037/a0020327. PMID&nbsp;20804236. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/46094267

  293. Millon T, Krueger R, Simonsen E (2011). Contemporary Directions in Psychopathology: Scientific Foundations of the DSM-IV and ICD-11. Guilford Press.

  294. De Bolle M, Beyers W, De Clercq B, De Fruyt F (November 2012). &quot;General personality and psychopathology in referred and nonreferred children and adolescents: an investigation of continuity, pathoplasty, and complication models&quot;. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 121 (4): 958&#8211;70. doi:10.1037/a0027742. PMID&nbsp;22448741. S2CID&nbsp;33228527. https://biblio.ugent.be/publication/2117776/file/6770996

  295. Millon T, Krueger R, Simonsen E (2011). Contemporary Directions in Psychopathology: Scientific Foundations of the DSM-IV and ICD-11. Guilford Press.

  296. Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D (September 2010). &quot;Linking &quot;big&quot; personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis&quot;. Psychological Bulletin. 136 (5): 768&#8211;821. doi:10.1037/a0020327. PMID&nbsp;20804236. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/46094267

  297. Ormel J, Jeronimus BF, Kotov R, Riese H, Bos EH, Hankin B, Rosmalen JG, Oldehinkel AJ (July 2013). &quot;Neuroticism and common mental disorders: meaning and utility of a complex relationship&quot;. Clinical Psychology Review. 33 (5): 686&#8211;97. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.04.003. PMC&nbsp;4382368. PMID&nbsp;23702592. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4382368

  298. Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D (September 2010). &quot;Linking &quot;big&quot; personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis&quot;. Psychological Bulletin. 136 (5): 768&#8211;821. doi:10.1037/a0020327. PMID&nbsp;20804236. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/46094267

  299. Ormel J, Jeronimus BF, Kotov R, Riese H, Bos EH, Hankin B, Rosmalen JG, Oldehinkel AJ (July 2013). &quot;Neuroticism and common mental disorders: meaning and utility of a complex relationship&quot;. Clinical Psychology Review. 33 (5): 686&#8211;97. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.04.003. PMC&nbsp;4382368. PMID&nbsp;23702592. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4382368

  300. Millon T, Krueger R, Simonsen E (2011). Contemporary Directions in Psychopathology: Scientific Foundations of the DSM-IV and ICD-11. Guilford Press.

  301. Krueger R, Tackett L (2006). Personality and Psychopathology. Guilford Press.[page&nbsp;needed][ISBN&nbsp;missing] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  302. Krueger R, Tackett L (2006). Personality and Psychopathology. Guilford Press.[page&nbsp;needed][ISBN&nbsp;missing] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  303. Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D (September 2010). &quot;Linking &quot;big&quot; personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis&quot;. Psychological Bulletin. 136 (5): 768&#8211;821. doi:10.1037/a0020327. PMID&nbsp;20804236. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/46094267

  304. Ormel J, Jeronimus BF, Kotov R, Riese H, Bos EH, Hankin B, Rosmalen JG, Oldehinkel AJ (July 2013). &quot;Neuroticism and common mental disorders: meaning and utility of a complex relationship&quot;. Clinical Psychology Review. 33 (5): 686&#8211;97. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.04.003. PMC&nbsp;4382368. PMID&nbsp;23702592. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4382368

  305. Millon T, Krueger R, Simonsen E (2011). Contemporary Directions in Psychopathology: Scientific Foundations of the DSM-IV and ICD-11. Guilford Press.

  306. Krueger R, Tackett L (2006). Personality and Psychopathology. Guilford Press.[page&nbsp;needed][ISBN&nbsp;missing] /wiki/Wikipedia:Citing_sources

  307. Millon T, Krueger R, Simonsen E (2011). Contemporary Directions in Psychopathology: Scientific Foundations of the DSM-IV and ICD-11. Guilford Press.

  308. Hudek-Knezevi&#263; J, Kardum I (August 2009). &quot;Five-factor personality dimensions and 3 health-related personality constructs as predictors of health&quot;. Croatian Medical Journal. 50 (4): 394&#8211;402. doi:10.3325/cmj.2009.50.394. PMC&nbsp;2728392. PMID&nbsp;19673040. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2728392

  309. Hudek-Knezevi&#263; J, Kardum I (August 2009). &quot;Five-factor personality dimensions and 3 health-related personality constructs as predictors of health&quot;. Croatian Medical Journal. 50 (4): 394&#8211;402. doi:10.3325/cmj.2009.50.394. PMC&nbsp;2728392. PMID&nbsp;19673040. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2728392

  310. Roberts BW, Kuncel NR, Shiner R, Caspi A, Goldberg LR (December 2007). &quot;The Power of Personality: The Comparative Validity of Personality Traits, Socioeconomic Status, and Cognitive Ability for Predicting Important Life Outcomes&quot; (PDF). Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2 (4): 313&#8211;45. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00047.x. PMC&nbsp;4499872. PMID&nbsp;26151971. http://jenni.uchicago.edu/Spencer_Conference/Representative%20Papers/Roberts%20et%20al,%202007%20PPS%20power%20of%20personality.pdf

  311. Jerram, Kathryn L.; Coleman, Peter G. (1999). &quot;The big five personality traits and reporting of health problems and health behaviour in old age&quot;. British Journal of Health Psychology. 4 (2): 181&#8211;92. doi:10.1348/135910799168560. ISSN&nbsp;2044-8287. https://bpspsychub.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1348/135910799168560

  312. Iwasa H, Masui Y, Gondo Y, Inagaki H, Kawaai C, Suzuki T (May 2008). &quot;Personality and all-cause mortality among older adults dwelling in a Japanese community: a five-year population-based prospective cohort study&quot;. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 16 (5): 399&#8211;405. doi:10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181662ac9. PMID&nbsp;18403571. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  313. Jokela M, Hintsanen M, Hakulinen C, Batty GD, Nabi H, Singh-Manoux A, Kivim&#228;ki M (April 2013). &quot;Association of personality with the development and persistence of obesity: a meta-analysis based on individual-participant data&quot;. Obesity Reviews. 14 (4): 315&#8211;23. doi:10.1111/obr.12007. PMC&nbsp;3717171. PMID&nbsp;23176713. /wiki/Archana_Singh-Manoux

  314. Komarraju M, Karau SJ, Schmeck RR, Avdic A (September 2011). &quot;The Big Five personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (4): 472&#8211;77. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-individual-differences_2011-09_51_4/page/472

  315. Zeidner M, Shani-Zinovich I (11 October 2011). &quot;Do academically gifted and nongifted students differ on the Big-Five and adaptive status? Some recent data and conclusions&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (5): 566&#8211;70. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.007. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-individual-differences_2011-10_51_5/page/566

  316. Komarraju M, Karau SJ, Schmeck RR, Avdic A (September 2011). &quot;The Big Five personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (4): 472&#8211;77. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-individual-differences_2011-09_51_4/page/472

  317. Mutlu, Tansu; Balbag, Zafer; Cemrek, Fatih (2010-01-01). &quot;The role of self-esteem, locus of control and big five personality traits in predicting hopelessness&quot;. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences. World Conference on Learning, Teaching and Administration Papers. 9: 1788&#8211;1792. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.12.401. ISSN&nbsp;1877-0428. https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.sbspro.2010.12.401

  318. Mutlu, Tansu; Balbag, Zafer; Cemrek, Fatih (2010-01-01). &quot;The role of self-esteem, locus of control and big five personality traits in predicting hopelessness&quot;. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences. World Conference on Learning, Teaching and Administration Papers. 9: 1788&#8211;1792. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.12.401. ISSN&nbsp;1877-0428. https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.sbspro.2010.12.401

  319. Singh AK (2012). &quot;Does trait predict psychological well-being among students of professional courses?&quot;. Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology. 38 (2): 234&#8211;41. /wiki/Journal_of_the_Indian_Academy_of_Applied_Psychology

  320. Klimstra TA, Luyckx K, Germeijs V, Meeus WH, Goossens L (March 2012). &quot;Personality traits and educational identity formation in late adolescents: longitudinal associations and academic progress&quot; (PDF). Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 41 (3): 346&#8211;61. doi:10.1007/s10964-011-9734-7. PMID&nbsp;22147120. S2CID&nbsp;33747401. https://pure.uvt.nl/ws/files/1461188/OntwikPsy_Klimstra_personality__JoYaA_2012.pdf

  321. Komarraju M, Karau SJ, Schmeck RR, Avdic A (September 2011). &quot;The Big Five personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (4): 472&#8211;77. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-individual-differences_2011-09_51_4/page/472

  322. Pashler H, McDaniel M, Rohrer D, Bjork R (December 2008). &quot;Learning Styles: Concepts and Evidence&quot;. Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 9 (3): 105&#8211;19. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6053.2009.01038.x. PMID&nbsp;26162104. https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1539-6053.2009.01038.x

  323. Zhang Lf (6 September 2001). &quot;Measuring thinking styles in addition to measuring personality traits?&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 33 (3): 445&#8211;58. doi:10.1016/s0191-8869(01)00166-0. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  324. Schmeck RR, Ribich F, Ramainah N (1997). &quot;Development of a Self-Report inventory for assessing Individual Differences in Learning Processes&quot;. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1 (3): 413&#8211;31. doi:10.1177/014662167700100310. S2CID&nbsp;143890188. http://purl.umn.edu/98563

  325. Komarraju M, Karau SJ, Schmeck RR, Avdic A (September 2011). &quot;The Big Five personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (4): 472&#8211;77. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-individual-differences_2011-09_51_4/page/472

  326. Komarraju, Meera; Karau, Steven J.; Schmeck, Ronald R.; Avdic, Alen (2011-09-01). &quot;The Big Five personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. Digit Ratio (2D:4D) and Individual Differences Research. 51 (4): 472&#8211;477. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019. ISSN&nbsp;0191-8869. https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0191886911002194

  327. Komarraju M, Karau SJ, Schmeck RR, Avdic A (September 2011). &quot;The Big Five personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (4): 472&#8211;77. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-individual-differences_2011-09_51_4/page/472

  328. Komarraju M, Karau SJ, Schmeck RR, Avdic A (September 2011). &quot;The Big Five personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (4): 472&#8211;77. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-individual-differences_2011-09_51_4/page/472

  329. Komarraju M, Karau SJ, Schmeck RR, Avdic A (September 2011). &quot;The Big Five personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (4): 472&#8211;77. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-individual-differences_2011-09_51_4/page/472

  330. Komarraju M, Karau SJ, Schmeck RR, Avdic A (September 2011). &quot;The Big Five personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (4): 472&#8211;77. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-individual-differences_2011-09_51_4/page/472

  331. Jensen, Mikael (2015). &quot;Personality Traits, Learning and Academic Achievements&quot;. Journal of Education and Learning. 4 (4): 91. doi:10.5539/jel.v4n4p91. https://doi.org/10.5539%2Fjel.v4n4p91

  332. Jensen, Mikael (2015). &quot;Personality Traits, Learning and Academic Achievements&quot;. Journal of Education and Learning. 4 (4): 91. doi:10.5539/jel.v4n4p91. https://doi.org/10.5539%2Fjel.v4n4p91

  333. Jensen, Mikael (2015). &quot;Personality Traits, Learning and Academic Achievements&quot;. Journal of Education and Learning. 4 (4): 91. doi:10.5539/jel.v4n4p91. https://doi.org/10.5539%2Fjel.v4n4p91

  334. Klimstra TA, Luyckx K, Germeijs V, Meeus WH, Goossens L (March 2012). &quot;Personality traits and educational identity formation in late adolescents: longitudinal associations and academic progress&quot; (PDF). Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 41 (3): 346&#8211;61. doi:10.1007/s10964-011-9734-7. PMID&nbsp;22147120. S2CID&nbsp;33747401. https://pure.uvt.nl/ws/files/1461188/OntwikPsy_Klimstra_personality__JoYaA_2012.pdf

  335. De Feyter T, Caers R, Vigna C, Berings D (22 March 2012). &quot;Unraveling the impact of the Big Five personality traits on academic performance: The moderating and mediating effects of self-efficacy and academic motivation&quot;. Learning and Individual Differences. 22 (4): 439&#8211;48. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.03.013. https://lirias.kuleuven.be/handle/123456789/409065

  336. Klimstra TA, Luyckx K, Germeijs V, Meeus WH, Goossens L (March 2012). &quot;Personality traits and educational identity formation in late adolescents: longitudinal associations and academic progress&quot; (PDF). Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 41 (3): 346&#8211;61. doi:10.1007/s10964-011-9734-7. PMID&nbsp;22147120. S2CID&nbsp;33747401. https://pure.uvt.nl/ws/files/1461188/OntwikPsy_Klimstra_personality__JoYaA_2012.pdf

  337. Komarraju M, Karau SJ, Schmeck RR, Avdic A (September 2011). &quot;The Big Five personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (4): 472&#8211;77. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-individual-differences_2011-09_51_4/page/472

  338. Vedel A (2014). &quot;The Big Five and tertiary academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis&quot; (PDF). Personality and Individual Differences. 71: 66&#8211;76. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.011. https://pure.au.dk/ws/files/112928639/The_Big_Five_and_tertiary_academic_performance_Postprint_2014.pdf

  339. Trapmann, S., Hell, B., Hirn, J.-O. W. ve Schuler, H. (2007). Meta-analysis of the relationship between the Big Five and academic success at university. Zeitschrift f&#252;r Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 215(2), 132&#8211;51.

  340. Bartolic-Zlomislic, Bates A (1999). &quot;Investing in On-line Learning: Potential Benefits and Limitations&quot;. Canadian Journal of Communication. 24 (3). doi:10.22230/CJC.1999V24N3A1111. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  341. Holland, J. L. (1966). The Psychology of Vocational Choice: A Theory of Personality Types and Model Environments. Oxford: Blaisdell.

  342. Armitage, Catherine (12 February 2020). &quot;Scientists are curious and passionate and ready to argue&quot;. Retrieved 9 June 2021. https://www.natureindex.com/news-blog/scientists-are-curious-and-idealistic-but-not-very-agreeable-compared-to-other-professions

  343. Vedel, Anna (2016). &quot;Big Five personality group differences across academic majors: A systematic review&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 92: 1&#8211;10. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.12.011. ISSN&nbsp;0191-8869. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.12.011

  344. Paunonen SV, Ashton MS (2001). &quot;Big Five factors and facets and the prediction of behavior&quot;. Journal of Personality &amp; Social Psychology. 81 (3): 524&#8211;39. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.3.524. PMID&nbsp;11554651. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  345. Mount MK, Barrick MR (1998). &quot;Five reasons why the &quot;big five&quot; article has been frequently cited&quot;. Personnel Psychology. 51 (4): 849&#8211;57. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1998.tb00743.x. https://archive.org/details/sim_personnel-psychology_winter-1998_51_4/page/849

  346. Morgeson FP, Campion MA, Dipboye RL, Hollenbeck JR, Murphy K, Schmitt N (2007). &quot;Reconsidering the use of personality tests in personnel selection contexts&quot;. Personnel Psychology. 60 (3): 683&#8211;729. CiteSeerX&nbsp;10.1.1.493.5981. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2007.00089.x. https://archive.org/details/sim_personnel-psychology_autumn-2007_60_3/page/683

  347. Mischel W (1968). Personality and assessment. London: Wiley. ISBN&nbsp;978-0-8058-2330-1. 978-0-8058-2330-1

  348. Rosenthal R (1990). &quot;How are we doing in soft psychology?&quot;. American Psychologist. 45 (6): 775&#8211;77. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.45.6.775. https://archive.org/details/sim_american-psychologist_1990-06_45_6/page/775

  349. Hunter JE, Schmidt FL, Judiesch MK (1990). &quot;Individual differences in output variability as a function of job complexity&quot;. Journal of Applied Psychology. 75: 28&#8211;42. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.75.1.28. S2CID&nbsp;144507523. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-applied-psychology_1990-02_75_1/page/28

  350. Judge TA, Bono JE, Ilies R, Gerhardt MW (August 2002). &quot;Personality and leadership: a qualitative and quantitative review&quot;. The Journal of Applied Psychology. 87 (4): 765&#8211;80. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.87.4.765. PMID&nbsp;12184579. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-applied-psychology_2002-08_87_4/page/765

  351. Spurk D, Abele AE (16 June 2010). &quot;Who Earns More and Why? A Multiple Mediation Model from Personality to Salary&quot;. Journal of Business and Psychology. 26: 87&#8211;103. doi:10.1007/s10869-010-9184-3. S2CID&nbsp;144290202. https://boris.unibe.ch/65687/

  352. McLean, Dawson; Bouaissa, Mohsen; Rainville, Bruno; Auger, Ludovic (2019-12-04). &quot;Non-Cognitive Skills: How Much Do They Matter for Earnings in Canada?&quot;. American Journal of Management. 19 (4). doi:10.33423/ajm.v19i4.2392. ISSN&nbsp;2165-7998. https://articlegateway.com/index.php/AJM/article/view/2392

  353. Mehta P (2012). &quot;Personality as a predictor of burnout among managers of manufacturing industries.&quot;. Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology. 32: 321&#8211;28.

  354. Fairweather J (2012). &quot;Personality, nations, and innovation: Relationships between personality traits and national innovation scores&quot;. Cross-Cultural Research. 46: 3&#8211;30. doi:10.1177/1069397111409124. S2CID&nbsp;144015495. https://archive.org/details/sim_cross-cultural-research_2012-02_46_1/page/3

  355. Spurk D, Abele AE (16 June 2010). &quot;Who Earns More and Why? A Multiple Mediation Model from Personality to Salary&quot;. Journal of Business and Psychology. 26: 87&#8211;103. doi:10.1007/s10869-010-9184-3. S2CID&nbsp;144290202. https://boris.unibe.ch/65687/

  356. Camps J, Stouten J, Euwema M (February 2016). &quot;The relation between supervisors' big five personality traits and employees' experiences of abusive supervision&quot;. Frontiers in Psychology. 10 (7): 112. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00112. PMC&nbsp;4748047. PMID&nbsp;26903919. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4748047

  357. Tepper BJ (June 2007). &quot;Abusive supervision in work organizations: Review, synthesis, and research agenda&quot;. Journal of Management. 33 (3): 261&#8211;89. doi:10.1177/0149206307300812. S2CID&nbsp;143934380. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  358. Judge &amp; LePine, &quot;Bright and Dark Sides&#8230;&quot; Research Companion to the Dysfunctional Workplace, 2007 | p. 332-355

  359. Judge &amp; LePine, &quot;Bright and Dark Sides&#8230;&quot; Research Companion to the Dysfunctional Workplace, 2007 | p. 332-355

  360. Sackett PR, Walmsley PT (2014). &quot;Which Personality Attributes Are Most Important in the Workplace?&quot;. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 9 (5): 538&#8211;51. doi:10.1177/1745691614543972. PMID&nbsp;26186756. S2CID&nbsp;21245818. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  361. Judge TA, Livingston BA, Hurst C (February 2012). &quot;Do nice guys--and gals--really finish last? The joint effects of sex and agreeableness on income&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 102 (2): 390&#8211;407. doi:10.1037/a0026021. PMID&nbsp;22121889. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_2012-02_102_2/page/390

  362. Judge &amp; LePine, &quot;Bright and Dark Sides&#8230;&quot; Research Companion to the Dysfunctional Workplace, 2007 | p. 332-355

  363. Sackett PR, Walmsley PT (2014). &quot;Which Personality Attributes Are Most Important in the Workplace?&quot;. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 9 (5): 538&#8211;51. doi:10.1177/1745691614543972. PMID&nbsp;26186756. S2CID&nbsp;21245818. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  364. Judge &amp; LePine, &quot;Bright and Dark Sides&#8230;&quot; Research Companion to the Dysfunctional Workplace, 2007 | p. 332-355

  365. Carter, Nathan T.; Miller, Joshua D.; Widiger, Thomas A. (2018-12-01). &quot;Extreme Personalities at Work and in Life&quot;. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 27 (6): 429&#8211;436. doi:10.1177/0963721418793134. ISSN&nbsp;0963-7214. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1177/0963721418793134

  366. Neal A, Yeo G, Koy A, Xiao T (26 January 2011). &quot;Predicting the Form and Direction of Work Role Performance From the Big 5 Model of Personality Traits&quot;. Journal of Organizational Behavior. 33 (2): 175&#8211;92. doi:10.1002/job.742. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  367. Judge &amp; LePine, &quot;Bright and Dark Sides&#8230;&quot; Research Companion to the Dysfunctional Workplace, 2007 | p. 332-355

  368. Neal A, Yeo G, Koy A, Xiao T (26 January 2011). &quot;Predicting the Form and Direction of Work Role Performance From the Big 5 Model of Personality Traits&quot;. Journal of Organizational Behavior. 33 (2): 175&#8211;92. doi:10.1002/job.742. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  369. Gavoille, Nicolas; Hazans, Mihails (2022). &quot;Personality Traits, Remote Work and Productivity&quot;. SSRN Electronic Journal. doi:10.2139/ssrn.4188297. hdl:10419/265707. ISSN&nbsp;1556-5068. https://www.ssrn.com/abstract=4188297

  370. Wright, Sandra (2023). Personality as a Predictor Of Job Performance in an All-Remote Workforce: A Study of Workers Within the Canada Pension Centre for the Federal Public Service (Doctor of Philosophy thesis). Carleton University. doi:10.22215/etd/2023-15519. hdl:20.500.14718/42842. https://repository.library.carleton.ca/concern/etds/jd472x515

  371. Olsen, Espen; Fu, Yusheng; Jensen, Maria (2024-07-05). &quot;The Influence of Remote Work on Personality Trait&#8211;Performance Linkages: A Two-Wave Longitudinal Study&quot;. Administrative Sciences. 14 (7): 144. doi:10.3390/admsci14070144. ISSN&nbsp;2076-3387. https://doi.org/10.3390%2Fadmsci14070144

  372. Parra, Carlos M.; Gupta, Manjul; Cadden, Trevor (November 2022). &quot;Towards an understanding of remote work exhaustion: A study on the effects of individuals' big five personality traits&quot;. Journal of Business Research. 150: 653&#8211;662. doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2022.06.009. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0148296322005422

  373. Weidmann, Rebekka; Ledermann, Thomas; Grob, Alexander (August 2017). &quot;Big Five traits and relationship satisfaction: The mediating role of self-esteem&quot;. Journal of Research in Personality. 69: 102&#8211;109. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2016.06.001. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0092656616300496

  374. Bach, Kathrin; Koch, Marco; Spinath, Frank M. (February 2025). &quot;Relationship satisfaction and The Big Five &#8211; Utilizing longitudinal data covering 9 years&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 233: 112887. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2024.112887. https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.paid.2024.112887

  375. O&#8217;Meara, Madison S.; South, Susan C. (December 2019). &quot;Big Five personality domains and relationship satisfaction: Direct effects and correlated change over time&quot;. Journal of Personality. 87 (6): 1206&#8211;1220. doi:10.1111/jopy.12468. ISSN&nbsp;0022-3506. PMC&nbsp;11239117. PMID&nbsp;30776092. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11239117

  376. Malouff, John M.; Thorsteinsson, Einar B.; Schutte, Nicola S.; Bhullar, Navjot; Rooke, Sally E. (February 2010). &quot;The Five-Factor Model of personality and relationship satisfaction of intimate partners: A meta-analysis&quot;. Journal of Research in Personality. 44 (1): 124&#8211;127. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2009.09.004. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0092656609002001

  377. O'Brien, Ruth (2008). &quot;Big five personality characteristics and commitment levels in romantic relationships&quot;. https://scholar.utc.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1152&context=mps

  378. Allen, Natalie J.; Meyer, John P. (March 1990). &quot;The measurement and antecedents of affective, continuance and normative commitment to the organization&quot;. Journal of Occupational Psychology. 63 (1): 1&#8211;18. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8325.1990.tb00506.x. ISSN&nbsp;0305-8107. https://bpspsychub.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.2044-8325.1990.tb00506.x

  379. Johnson, Michael P. (1999), Adams, Jeffrey M.; Jones, Warren H. (eds.), &quot;Personal, Moral, and Structural Commitment to Relationships&quot;, Handbook of Interpersonal Commitment and Relationship Stability, Boston, MA: Springer US, pp.&nbsp;73&#8211;87, doi:10.1007/978-1-4615-4773-0_4, ISBN&nbsp;978-1-4613-7161-8, retrieved 2025-05-07 978-1-4613-7161-8

  380. Johnson, Michael P.; Caughlin, John P.; Huston, Ted L. (February 1999). &quot;The Tripartite Nature of Marital Commitment: Personal, Moral, and Structural Reasons to Stay Married&quot;. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 61 (1): 160. doi:10.2307/353891. JSTOR&nbsp;353891. https://www.jstor.org/stable/353891

  381. Allen, Natalie J.; Meyer, John P. (March 1990). &quot;The measurement and antecedents of affective, continuance and normative commitment to the organization&quot;. Journal of Occupational Psychology. 63 (1): 1&#8211;18. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8325.1990.tb00506.x. ISSN&nbsp;0305-8107. https://bpspsychub.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.2044-8325.1990.tb00506.x

  382. Asselmann, Eva; Specht, Jule (September 2020). &quot;Taking the ups and downs at the rollercoaster of love: Associations between major life events in the domain of romantic relationships and the Big Five personality traits&quot;. Developmental Psychology. 56 (9): 1803&#8211;1816. doi:10.1037/dev0001047. ISSN&nbsp;1939-0599. PMID&nbsp;32672996. https://doi.apa.org/doi/10.1037/dev0001047

  383. Heller, Daniel; Watson, David; Ilies, Remus (July 2004). &quot;The Role of Person Versus Situation in Life Satisfaction: A Critical Examination&quot;. Psychological Bulletin. 130 (4): 574&#8211;600. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.574. ISSN&nbsp;1939-1455. PMID&nbsp;15250814. https://doi.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.574

  384. Asselmann, Eva; Specht, Jule (September 2020). &quot;Taking the ups and downs at the rollercoaster of love: Associations between major life events in the domain of romantic relationships and the Big Five personality traits&quot;. Developmental Psychology. 56 (9): 1803&#8211;1816. doi:10.1037/dev0001047. ISSN&nbsp;1939-0599. PMID&nbsp;32672996. https://doi.apa.org/doi/10.1037/dev0001047

  385. Neyer, Franz J.; Lehnart, Judith (June 2007). &quot;Relationships Matter in Personality Development: Evidence From an 8-Year Longitudinal Study Across Young Adulthood&quot;. Journal of Personality. 75 (3): 535&#8211;568. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00448.x. ISSN&nbsp;0022-3506. PMID&nbsp;17489891. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00448.x

  386. Gerber AS, et&nbsp;al. (2010). &quot;Personality and Political Attitudes: Relationships across Issue Domains and Political Contexts&quot;. The American Political Science Review. 104: 111&#8211;133. doi:10.1017/S0003055410000031. S2CID&nbsp;6208090. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  387. Sweetser KD (2014). &quot;Partisan Personality: The Psychological Differences Between Democrats and Republicans, and Independents Somewhere in Between&quot;. American Behavioral Scientist. 58 (9): 1183&#8211;94. doi:10.1177/0002764213506215. S2CID&nbsp;145674720. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  388. Fatke M (2017). &quot;Personality Traits and Political Ideology: A First Global Assessment&quot;. Political Psychology. 38 (5): 881&#8211;99. doi:10.1111/pops.12347. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  389. Gerber AS, et&nbsp;al. (2010). &quot;Personality and Political Attitudes: Relationships across Issue Domains and Political Contexts&quot;. The American Political Science Review. 104: 111&#8211;133. doi:10.1017/S0003055410000031. S2CID&nbsp;6208090. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  390. Bakker BN, et&nbsp;al. (2015). &quot;Personality Traits and Party Identification over Time&quot;. European Journal of Political Research. 54 (2): 197&#8211;215. doi:10.1111/1475-6765.12070. https://dare.uva.nl/personal/pure/en/publications/personality-traits-and-party-identification-over-time(7d835c89-e58f-4be4-bb01-621610c77f97).html

  391. Gerber AS, et&nbsp;al. (2012). &quot;Personality and the Strength and Direction of Partisan Identification&quot;. Political Behavior. 34 (4): 653&#8211;688. doi:10.1007/s11109-011-9178-5. S2CID&nbsp;144317734. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  392. Bakker BN, et&nbsp;al. (2015). &quot;Personality Traits and Party Identification over Time&quot;. European Journal of Political Research. 54 (2): 197&#8211;215. doi:10.1111/1475-6765.12070. https://dare.uva.nl/personal/pure/en/publications/personality-traits-and-party-identification-over-time(7d835c89-e58f-4be4-bb01-621610c77f97).html

  393. Gerber AS, et&nbsp;al. (2012). &quot;Personality and the Strength and Direction of Partisan Identification&quot;. Political Behavior. 34 (4): 653&#8211;688. doi:10.1007/s11109-011-9178-5. S2CID&nbsp;144317734. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  394. L&#246;we, Konstantin Felix. &quot;Is Politics Downstream from Personality? The Five Factor Model's Effect on Political Orientation in Sweden.&quot; (2019). http://lup.lub.lu.se/student-papers/record/8992021 Thesis http://lup.lub.lu.se/student-papers/record/8992021

  395. Jeronimus BF, Kotov R, Riese H, Ormel J (October 2016). &quot;Neuroticism's prospective association with mental disorders halves after adjustment for baseline symptoms and psychiatric history, but the adjusted association hardly decays with time: a meta-analysis on 59 longitudinal/prospective studies with 443 313 participants&quot;. Psychological Medicine. 46 (14): 2883&#8211;906. doi:10.1017/S0033291716001653. PMID&nbsp;27523506. S2CID&nbsp;23548727. https://zenodo.org/record/895885

  396. Trofimova I, Robbins TW, Sulis WH, Uher J (April 2018). &quot;Taxonomies of psychological individual differences: biological perspectives on millennia-long challenges&quot;. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 373 (1744): 20170152. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0152. PMC&nbsp;5832678. PMID&nbsp;29483338. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5832678

  397. Roberts, p. 338

  398. Saroglou, Vassilis (2002). &quot;Religion and the five-factors of personality: A meta-analytic review&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 32: 15&#8211;25. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00233-6. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  399. &quot;IPIP Home&quot;. ipip.ori.org. Retrieved 2017-07-01. http://ipip.ori.org/

  400. Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ, Swann WB (2003). &quot;A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains&quot;. Journal of Research in Personality. 37 (6): 504&#8211;28. CiteSeerX&nbsp;10.1.1.1013.6925. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1. ISSN&nbsp;0092-6566. S2CID&nbsp;7147133. /wiki/CiteSeerX_(identifier)

  401. De Fruyt F, McCrae RR, Szirm&#225;k Z, Nagy J (September 2004). &quot;The Five-factor Personality Inventory as a measure of the Five-factor Model: Belgian, American, and Hungarian comparisons with the NEO-PI-R&quot;. Assessment. 11 (3): 207&#8211;15. doi:10.1177/1073191104265800. PMID&nbsp;15358876. S2CID&nbsp;29733250. https://archive.org/details/sim_assessment_2004-09_11_3/page/207

  402. Goldberg LR (1992). &quot;The development of markers for the Big-five factor structure&quot;. Psychological Assessment. 4 (1): 26&#8211;42. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.26. S2CID&nbsp;144709415. https://archive.org/details/sim_psychological-assessment_1992-03_4_1/page/26

  403. Donaldson SI, Grant-Vallone EJ (2002). &quot;Understanding self-report bias in organizational behavior research&quot;. Journal of Business and Psychology. 17 (2): 245&#8211;60. doi:10.1023/A:1019637632584. JSTOR&nbsp;25092818. S2CID&nbsp;10464760. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  404. Hirsh JB, Peterson JB (October 2008). &quot;Predicting creativity and academic success with a 'Fake-Proof' measure of the Big Five&quot;. Journal of Research in Personality. 42 (5): 1323&#8211;33. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.04.006. S2CID&nbsp;18849547. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  405. De Fruyt F, McCrae RR, Szirm&#225;k Z, Nagy J (September 2004). &quot;The Five-factor Personality Inventory as a measure of the Five-factor Model: Belgian, American, and Hungarian comparisons with the NEO-PI-R&quot;. Assessment. 11 (3): 207&#8211;15. doi:10.1177/1073191104265800. PMID&nbsp;15358876. S2CID&nbsp;29733250. https://archive.org/details/sim_assessment_2004-09_11_3/page/207

  406. Goldberg LR (1992). &quot;The development of markers for the Big-five factor structure&quot;. Psychological Assessment. 4 (1): 26&#8211;42. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.26. S2CID&nbsp;144709415. https://archive.org/details/sim_psychological-assessment_1992-03_4_1/page/26

  407. Thompson ER (2008). &quot;Development and validation of an international English big-five mini-markers&quot;. Personality and Individual Differences. 45 (6): 542&#8211;48. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.06.013. https://archive.org/details/sim_personality-and-individual-differences_2008-10_45_6/page/542

  408. Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ, Swann WB (2003). &quot;A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains&quot;. Journal of Research in Personality. 37 (6): 504&#8211;28. CiteSeerX&nbsp;10.1.1.1013.6925. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1. ISSN&nbsp;0092-6566. S2CID&nbsp;7147133. /wiki/CiteSeerX_(identifier)

  409. Cred&#233; M, Harms P, Niehorster S, Gaye-Valentine A (April 2012). &quot;An evaluation of the consequences of using short measures of the Big Five personality traits&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 102 (4): 874&#8211;88. doi:10.1037/a0027403. PMID&nbsp;22352328. http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1089&context=managementfacpub

  410. Tkachenko, Yegor; Jedidi, Kamel; Ansari, Asim (2022). &quot;Reining in Long Consumer Questionnaires with Self-Supervised Deep Reinforcement Learning&quot; (PDF). Wharton JMP. https://marketing.wharton.upenn.edu/wp-content/uploads/2022/10/Tkachenko-Yegor-JMP-Paper.pdf

  411. Goldberg LR (December 1990). &quot;An alternative &quot;description of personality&quot;: the big-five factor structure&quot;. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 59 (6): 1216&#8211;29. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1216. PMID&nbsp;2283588. S2CID&nbsp;9034636. https://archive.org/details/sim_journal-of-personality-and-social-psychology_1990-12_59_6/page/1216

  412. Donaldson SI, Grant-Vallone EJ (2002). &quot;Understanding self-report bias in organizational behavior research&quot;. Journal of Business and Psychology. 17 (2): 245&#8211;60. doi:10.1023/A:1019637632584. JSTOR&nbsp;25092818. S2CID&nbsp;10464760. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  413. McFarland, Lynn A.; Ryan, Ann Marie (2000). &quot;Variance in faking across noncognitive measures&quot;. Journal of Applied Psychology. 85 (5): 812&#8211;21. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.85.5.812. ISSN&nbsp;1939-1854. PMID&nbsp;11055152. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  414. &quot;Big Five Personality Tests, traits and background&quot;. Personality and Aptitude Career Tests. Retrieved 2017-07-01. http://www.personality-and-aptitude-career-tests.com/big-five-personality-tests.html

  415. Hirsh JB, Peterson JB (October 2008). &quot;Predicting creativity and academic success with a 'Fake-Proof' measure of the Big Five&quot;. Journal of Research in Personality. 42 (5): 1323&#8211;33. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.04.006. S2CID&nbsp;18849547. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  416. Schwartz HA, Eichstaedt JC, Kern ML, Dziurzynski L, Ramones SM, Agrawal M, Shah A, Kosinski M, Stillwell D, Seligman ME, Ungar LH (2013). &quot;Personality, gender, and age in the language of social media: the open-vocabulary approach&quot;. PLOS ONE. 8 (9): e73791. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...873791S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0073791. PMC&nbsp;3783449. PMID&nbsp;24086296. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3783449