Menu
Home Explore People Places Arts History Plants & Animals Science Life & Culture Technology
On this page
Metascience
Use of scientific methodology to study science itself

Metascience (also known as meta-research) is the use of scientific methodology to study science itself. Metascience seeks to increase the quality of scientific research while reducing inefficiency. It is also known as "research on research" and "the science of science", as it uses research methods to study how research is done and find where improvements can be made. Metascience concerns itself with all fields of research and has been described as "a bird's eye view of science". In the words of John Ioannidis, "Science is the best thing that has happened to human beings ... but we can do it better."

In 1966, an early meta-research paper examined the statistical methods of 295 papers published in ten high-profile medical journals. It found that "in almost 73% of the reports read ... conclusions were drawn when the justification for these conclusions was invalid." Meta-research in the following decades found many methodological flaws, inefficiencies, and poor practices in research across numerous scientific fields. Many scientific studies could not be reproduced, particularly in medicine and the soft sciences. The term "replication crisis" was coined in the early 2010s as part of a growing awareness of the problem.

Measures have been implemented to address the issues revealed by metascience. These measures include the pre-registration of scientific studies and clinical trials as well as the founding of organizations such as CONSORT and the EQUATOR Network that issue guidelines for methodology and reporting. There are continuing efforts to reduce the misuse of statistics, to eliminate perverse incentives from academia, to improve the peer review process, to systematically collect data about the scholarly publication system, to combat bias in scientific literature, and to increase the overall quality and efficiency of the scientific process. As such, metascience is a big part of methods underlying the Open Science Movement.

History

In 1966, an early meta-research paper examined the statistical methods of 295 papers published in ten high-profile medical journals. It found that, "in almost 73% of the reports read ... conclusions were drawn when the justification for these conclusions was invalid."6 A paper in 1976 called for funding for meta-research: "Because the very nature of research on research, particularly if it is prospective, requires long periods of time, we recommend that independent, highly competent groups be established with ample, long term support to conduct and support retrospective and prospective research on the nature of scientific discovery".7 In 2005, John Ioannidis published a paper titled "Why Most Published Research Findings Are False", which argued that a majority of papers in the medical field produce conclusions that are wrong.8 The paper went on to become the most downloaded paper in the Public Library of Science910 and is considered foundational to the field of metascience.11 In a related study with Jeremy Howick and Despina Koletsi, Ioannidis showed that only a minority of medical interventions are supported by 'high quality' evidence according to The Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) approach.12 Later meta-research identified widespread difficulty in replicating results in many scientific fields, including psychology and medicine. This problem was termed "the replication crisis". Metascience has grown as a reaction to the replication crisis and to concerns about waste in research.13

Many prominent publishers are interested in meta-research and in improving the quality of their publications. Top journals such as Science, The Lancet, and Nature, provide ongoing coverage of meta-research and problems with reproducibility.14 In 2012 PLOS ONE launched a Reproducibility Initiative. In 2015 Biomed Central introduced a minimum-standards-of-reporting checklist to four titles.

The first international conference in the broad area of meta-research was the Research Waste/EQUATOR conference held in Edinburgh in 2015; the first international conference on peer review was the Peer Review Congress held in 1989.15 In 2016, Research Integrity and Peer Review was launched. The journal's opening editorial called for "research that will increase our understanding and suggest potential solutions to issues related to peer review, study reporting, and research and publication ethics".16

Fields and topics of meta-research

Metascience can be categorized into five major areas of interest: Methods, Reporting, Reproducibility, Evaluation, and Incentives. These correspond, respectively, with how to perform, communicate, verify, evaluate, and reward research.17

Methods

Metascience seeks to identify poor research practices, including biases in research, poor study design, abuse of statistics, and to find methods to reduce these practices.18 Meta-research has identified numerous biases in scientific literature.19 Of particular note is the widespread misuse of p-values and abuse of statistical significance.2021

Scientific data science

Scientific data science is the use of data science to analyse research papers. It encompasses both qualitative and quantitative methods. Research in scientific data science includes fraud detection22 and citation network analysis.23

Journalology

Main article: Journalology

Journalology, also known as publication science, is the scholarly study of all aspects of the academic publishing process.2425 The field seeks to improve the quality of scholarly research by implementing evidence-based practices in academic publishing.26 The term "journalology" was coined by Stephen Lock, the former editor-in-chief of The BMJ. The first Peer Review Congress, held in 1989 in Chicago, Illinois, is considered a pivotal moment in the founding of journalology as a distinct field.27 The field of journalology has been influential in pushing for study pre-registration in science, particularly in clinical trials. Clinical-trial registration is now expected in most countries.28

Reporting

Meta-research has identified poor practices in reporting, explaining, disseminating and popularizing research, particularly within the social and health sciences. Poor reporting makes it difficult to accurately interpret the results of scientific studies, to replicate studies, and to identify biases and conflicts of interest in the authors. Solutions include the implementation of reporting standards, and greater transparency in scientific studies (including better requirements for disclosure of conflicts of interest). There is an attempt to standardize reporting of data and methodology through the creation of guidelines by reporting agencies such as CONSORT and the larger EQUATOR Network.29

Reproducibility

Further information: Replication crisis and Reproducibility

The replication crisis is an ongoing methodological crisis in which it has been found that many scientific studies are difficult or impossible to replicate.3031 While the crisis has its roots in the meta-research of the mid- to late 20th century, the phrase "replication crisis" was not coined until the early 2010s32 as part of a growing awareness of the problem.33 The replication crisis has been closely studied in psychology (especially social psychology) and medicine,3435 including cancer research.3637 Replication is an essential part of the scientific process, and the widespread failure of replication puts into question the reliability of affected fields.38

Moreover, replication of research (or failure to replicate) is considered less influential than original research, and is less likely to be published in many fields. This discourages the reporting of, and even attempts to replicate, studies.3940

Evaluation and incentives

See also: Academic journal § Prestige and ranking

Metascience seeks to create a scientific foundation for peer review. Meta-research evaluates peer review systems including pre-publication peer review, post-publication peer review, and open peer review. It also seeks to develop better research funding criteria.41

Metascience seeks to promote better research through better incentive systems. This includes studying the accuracy, effectiveness, costs, and benefits of different approaches to ranking and evaluating research and those who perform it.42 Critics argue that perverse incentives have created a publish-or-perish environment in academia which promotes the production of junk science, low quality research, and false positives.4344 According to Brian Nosek, "The problem that we face is that the incentive system is focused almost entirely on getting research published, rather than on getting research right."45 Proponents of reform seek to structure the incentive system to favor higher-quality results.46 For example, by quality being judged on the basis of narrative expert evaluations ("rather than [only or mainly] indices"), institutional evaluation criteria, guaranteeing of transparency, and professional standards.47

Contributorship

Studies proposed machine-readable standards and (a taxonomy of) badges for science publication management systems that hones in on contributorship – who has contributed what and how much of the research labor – rather that using traditional concept of plain authorship – who was involved in any way creation of a publication.48495051 A study pointed out one of the problems associated with the ongoing neglect of contribution nuanciation – it found that "the number of publications has ceased to be a good metric as a result of longer author lists, shorter papers, and surging publication numbers".52

Assessment factors

Factors other than a submission's merits can substantially influence peer reviewers' evaluations.53 Such factors may however also be important such as the use of track-records about the veracity of a researchers' prior publications and its alignment with public interests. Nevertheless, evaluation systems – include those of peer-review – may substantially lack mechanisms and criteria that are oriented or well-performingly oriented towards merit, real-world positive impact, progress and public usefulness rather than analytical indicators such as number of citations or altmetrics even when such can be used as partial indicators of such ends.5455 Rethinking of the academic reward structure "to offer more formal recognition for intermediate products, such as data" could have positive impacts and reduce data withholding.56

Recognition of training

A commentary noted that academic rankings don't consider where (country and institute) the respective researchers were trained.57

Scientometrics

Main article: Scientometrics

Scientometrics concerns itself with measuring bibliographic data in scientific publications. Major research issues include the measurement of the impact of research papers and academic journals, the understanding of scientific citations, and the use of such measurements in policy and management contexts.58 Studies suggest that "metrics used to measure academic success, such as the number of publications, citation number, and impact factor, have not changed for decades" and have to some degrees "ceased" to be good measures,5960 leading to issues such as "overproduction, unnecessary fragmentations, overselling, predatory journals (pay and publish), clever plagiarism, and deliberate obfuscation of scientific results so as to sell and oversell".61

Novel tools in this area include systems to quantify how much the cited-node informs the citing-node.62 This can be used to convert unweighted citation networks to a weighted one and then for importance assessment, deriving "impact metrics for the various entities involved, like the publications, authors etc"63 as well as, among other tools, for search engine- and recommendation systems.

Science governance

See also: Science policy and Science of science policy

Science funding and science governance can also be explored and informed by metascience.64

Incentives

Various interventions such as prioritization can be important. For instance, the concept of differential technological development refers to deliberately developing technologies – e.g. control-, safety- and policy-technologies versus risky biotechnologies – at different precautionary paces to decrease risks, mainly global catastrophic risk, by influencing the sequence in which technologies are developed.6566 Relying only on the established form of legislation and incentives to ensure the right outcomes may not be adequate as these may often be too slow67 or inappropriate.

Other incentives to govern science and related processes, including via metascience-based reforms, may include ensuring accountability to the public (in terms of e.g. accessibility of, especially publicly-funded, research or of it addressing various research topics of public interest in serious manners), increasing the qualified productive scientific workforce, improving the efficiency of science to improve problem-solving in general, and facilitating that unambiguous societal needs based on solid scientific evidence – such as about human physiology – are adequately prioritized and addressed. Such interventions, incentives and intervention-designs can be subjects of metascience.

Science funding and awards

See also: Patent § Criticism

Scientific awards are one category of science incentives. Metascience can explore existing and hypothetical systems of science awards. For instance, it found that work honored by Nobel Prizes clustered in only a few scientific fields with only 36/71 having received at least one Nobel Prize. Of the 114/849 domains science could be divided into their DC2 and DC3 classification systems, five were shown to comprise over half of the Nobel Prizes awarded between 1995 and 2017 (particle physics [14%], cell biology [12.1%], atomic physics [10.9%], neuroscience [10.1%], molecular chemistry [5.3%]).6869

A study found that delegation of responsibility by policy-makers – a centralized authority-based top-down approach – for knowledge production and appropriate funding to science with science subsequently somehow delivering "reliable and useful knowledge to society" is too simple.70

Measurements show that allocation of bio-medical resources can be more strongly correlated to previous allocations and research than to burden of diseases.71

A study suggests that "[i]f peer review is maintained as the primary mechanism of arbitration in the competitive selection of research reports and funding, then the scientific community needs to make sure it is not arbitrary".72

Studies indicate there to is a need to "reconsider how we measure success" (see #Factors of success and progress).73

Funding data

Funding information from grant databases and funding acknowledgment sections can be sources of data for scientometrics studies, e.g. for investigating or recognition of the impact of funding entities on the development of science and technology.74

Research questions and coordination

Main article: Research question § Aggregated research questions and coordination

Risk governance

Main article: Biosecurity § Future

Science communication and public use

See also: Science § Science and the public

It has been argued that "science has two fundamental attributes that underpin its value as a global public good: that knowledge claims and the evidence on which they are based are made openly available to scrutiny, and that the results of scientific research are communicated promptly and efficiently".75 Metascientific research is exploring topics of science communication such as media coverage of science, science journalism and online communication of results by science educators and scientists.76777879 A study found that the "main incentive academics are offered for using social media is amplification" and that it should be "moving towards an institutional culture that focuses more on how these [or such] platforms can facilitate real engagement with research".80 Science communication may also involve the communication of societal needs, concerns and requests to scientists.

Alternative metrics tools

Alternative metrics tools can be used not only for help in assessment (performance and impact)81 and findability, but also aggregate many of the public discussions about a scientific paper in social media such as reddit, citations on Wikipedia, and reports about the study in the news media which can then in turn be analyzed in metascience or provided and used by related tools.82 In terms of assessment and findability, altmetrics rate publications' performance or impact by the interactions they receive through social media or other online platforms,83 which can for example be used for sorting recent studies by measured impact, including before other studies are citing them. The specific procedures of established altmetrics are not transparent84 and the used algorithms can not be customized or altered by the user as open source software can. A study has described various limitations of altmetrics and points "toward avenues for continued research and development".85 They are also limited in their use as a primary tool for researchers to find received constructive feedback. (see above)

Societal implications and applications

It has been suggested that it may benefit science if "intellectual exchange—particularly regarding the societal implications and applications of science and technology—are better appreciated and incentivized in the future".86

Knowledge integration

Primary studies "without context, comparison or summary are ultimately of limited value" and various types[additional citation(s) needed] of research syntheses and summaries integrate primary studies.87 Progress in key social-ecological challenges of the global environmental agenda is "hampered by a lack of integration and synthesis of existing scientific evidence", with a "fast-increasing volume of data", compartmentalized information and generally unmet evidence synthesis challenges.88 According to Khalil, researchers are facing the problem of too many papers – e.g. in March 2014 more than 8,000 papers were submitted to arXiv – and to "keep up with the huge amount of literature, researchers use reference manager software, they make summaries and notes, and they rely on review papers to provide an overview of a particular topic". He notes that review papers are usually (only)" for topics in which many papers were written already, and they can get outdated quickly" and suggests "wiki-review papers" that get continuously updated with new studies on a topic and summarize many studies' results and suggest future research.89 A study suggests that if a scientific publication is being cited in a Wikipedia article this could potentially be considered as an indicator of some form of impact for this publication,90 for example as this may, over time, indicate that the reference has contributed to a high-level of summary of the given topic.

Science journalism

Science journalists play an important role in the scientific ecosystem and in science communication to the public and need to "know how to use, relevant information when deciding whether to trust a research finding, and whether and how to report on it", vetting the findings that get transmitted to the public.91

Science education

Some studies investigate science education, e.g. the teaching about selected scientific controversies92 and historical discovery process of major scientific conclusions,93 and common scientific misconceptions.94 Education can also be a topic more generally such as how to improve the quality of scientific outputs and reduce the time needed before scientific work or how to enlarge and retain various scientific workforces.

Science misconceptions and anti-science attitudes

Further information: Science § Anti-science attitudes, Media literacy, and Misinformation § Countermeasures

Many students have misconceptions about what science is and how it works.95 Anti-science attitudes and beliefs are also a subject of research.9697 Hotez suggests antiscience "has emerged as a dominant and highly lethal force, and one that threatens global security", and that there is a need for "new infrastructure" that mitigates it.98

Evolution of sciences

Scientific practice

Metascience can investigate how scientific processes evolve over time. A study found that teams are growing in size, "increasing by an average of 17% per decade".99 (see labor advantage below)

It was found that prevalent forms of non-open access publication and prices charged for many conventional journals – even for publicly funded papers – are unwarranted, unnecessary – or suboptimal – and detrimental barriers to scientific progress.100101102103 Open access can save considerable amounts of financial resources, which could be used otherwise, and level the playing field for researchers in developing countries.104 There are substantial expenses for subscriptions, gaining access to specific studies, and for article processing charges. Paywall: The Business of Scholarship is a documentary on such issues.105

Another topic are the established styles of scientific communication (e.g. long text-form studies and reviews) and the scientific publishing practices – there are concerns about a "glacial pace" of conventional publishing.106 The use of preprint-servers to publish study-drafts early is increasing and open peer review,107 new tools to screen studies,108 and improved matching of submitted manuscripts to reviewers109 are among the proposals to speed up publication.

Science overall and intrafield developments

Studies have various kinds of metadata which can be utilized, complemented and made accessible in useful ways. OpenAlex is a free online index of over 200 million scientific documents that integrates and provides metadata such as sources, citations, author information, scientific fields and research topics. Its API and open source website can be used for metascience, scientometrics and novel tools that query this semantic web of papers.110111112 Another project under development, Scholia, uses metadata of scientific publications for various visualizations and aggregation features such as providing a simple user interface summarizing literature about a specific feature of the SARS-CoV-2 virus using Wikidata's "main subject" property.113

Subject-level resolutions

Beyond metadata explicitly assigned to studies by humans, natural language processing and AI can be used to assign research publications to topics – one study investigating the impact of science awards used such to associate a paper's text (not just keywords) with the linguistic content of Wikipedia's scientific topics pages ("pages are created and updated by scientists and users through crowdsourcing"), creating meaningful and plausible classifications of high-fidelity scientific topics for further analysis or navigability.114

Growth or stagnation of science overall

Further information: Scientific method § History, and Philosophy of science

Metascience research is investigating the growth of science overall, using e.g. data on the number of publications in bibliographic databases. A study found segments with different growth rates appear related to phases of "economic (e.g., industrialization)" – money is considered as necessary input to the science system – "and/or political developments (e.g., Second World War)". It also confirmed a recent exponential growth in the volume of scientific literature and calculated an average doubling period of 17.3 years.115

However, others have pointed out that is difficult to measure scientific progress in meaningful ways, partly because it's hard to accurately evaluate how important any given scientific discovery is. A variety of perspectives of the trajectories of science overall (impact, number of major discoveries, etc) have been described in books and articles, including that science is becoming harder (per dollar or hour spent), that if science "slowing today, it is because science has remained too focused on established fields", that papers and patents are increasingly less likely to be "disruptive" in terms of breaking with the past as measured by the "CD index",116 and that there is a great stagnation – possibly as part of a larger trend117 – whereby e.g. "things haven't changed nearly as much since the 1970s" when excluding the computer and the Internet.

Better understanding of potential slowdowns according to some measures could be a major opportunity to improve humanity's future.118 For example, emphasis on citations in the measurement of scientific productivity, information overloads,119 reliance on a narrower set of existing knowledge (which may include narrow specialization and related contemporary practices) based on three "use of previous knowledge"-indicators,120 and risk-avoidant funding structures121 may have "toward incremental science and away from exploratory projects that are more likely to fail".122 The study that introduced the "CD index" suggests the overall number of papers has risen while the total of "highly disruptive" papers as measured by the index hasn't (notably, the 1998 discovery of the accelerating expansion of the universe has a CD index of 0). Their results also suggest scientists and inventors "may be struggling to keep up with the pace of knowledge expansion".123124

Various ways of measuring "novelty" of studies, novelty metrics,125 have been proposed to balance a potential anti-novelty bias – such as textual analysis126 or measuring whether it makes first-time-ever combinations of referenced journals, taking into account the difficulty.127 Other approaches include pro-actively funding risky projects.128 (see above)

Topic mapping

Science maps could show main interrelated topics within a certain scientific domain, their change over time, and their key actors (researchers, institutions, journals). They may help find factors determine the emergence of new scientific fields and the development of interdisciplinary areas and could be relevant for science policy purposes.129 (see above) Theories of scientific change could guide "the exploration and interpretation of visualized intellectual structures and dynamic patterns".130 The maps can show the intellectual, social or conceptual structure of a research field.131 Beyond visual maps, expert survey-based studies and similar approaches could identify understudied or neglected societally important areas, topic-level problems (such as stigma or dogma), or potential misprioritizations.[additional citation(s) needed] Examples of such are studies about policy in relation to public health132 and the social science of climate change mitigation133 where it has been estimated that only 0.12% of all funding for climate-related research is spent on such despite the most urgent puzzle at the current juncture being working out how to mitigate climate change, whereas the natural science of climate change is already well established.134

There are also studies that map a scientific field or a topic such as the study of the use of research evidence in policy and practice, partly using surveys.135

Controversies, current debates and disagreement

Some research is investigating scientific controversy or controversies, and may identify currently ongoing major debates (e.g. open questions), and disagreement between scientists or studies.[additional citation(s) needed] One study suggests the level of disagreement was highest in the social sciences and humanities (0.61%), followed by biomedical and health sciences (0.41%), life and earth sciences (0.29%); physical sciences and engineering (0.15%), and mathematics and computer science (0.06%).136 Such research may also show, where the disagreements are, especially if they cluster, including visually such as with cluster diagrams.

Challenges of interpretation of pooled results

See also: Clinical trial § Trial design

Further information: Meta-analysis § Challenges

Studies about a specific research question or research topic are often reviewed in the form of higher-level overviews in which results from various studies are integrated, compared, critically analyzed and interpreted. Examples of such works are scientific reviews and meta-analyses. These and related practices face various challenges and are a subject of metascience.

Various issues with included or available studies such as, for example, heterogeneity of methods used may lead to faulty conclusions of the meta-analysis.137

Knowledge integration and living documents

Various problems require swift integration of new and existing science-based knowledge. Especially setting where there are a large number of loosely related projects and initiatives benefit from a common ground or "commons".138

Evidence synthesis can be applied to important and, notably, both relatively urgent and certain global challenges: "climate change, energy transitions, biodiversity loss, antimicrobial resistance, poverty eradication and so on". It was suggested that a better system would keep summaries of research evidence up to date via living systematic reviews – e.g. as living documents. While the number of scientific papers and data (or information and online knowledge) has risen substantially,[additional citation(s) needed] the number of published academic systematic reviews has risen from "around 6,000 in 2011 to more than 45,000 in 2021".139 An evidence-based approach is important for progress in science, policy, medical and other practices. For example, meta-analyses can quantify what is known and identify what is not yet known140 and place "truly innovative and highly interdisciplinary ideas" into the context of established knowledge which may enhance their impact.141 (see above)

Factors of success and progress

It has been hypothesized that a deeper understanding of factors behind successful science could "enhance prospects of science as a whole to more effectively address societal problems".142143

Novel ideas and disruptive scholarship

Two metascientists reported that "structures fostering disruptive scholarship and focusing attention on novel ideas" could be important as in a growing scientific field citation flows disproportionately consolidate to already well-cited papers, possibly slowing and inhibiting canonical progress.144145 A study concluded that to enhance impact of truly innovative and highly interdisciplinary novel ideas, they should be placed in the context of established knowledge.146

Mentorship, partnerships and social factors

Other researchers reported that the most successful – in terms of "likelihood of prizewinning, National Academy of Science (NAS) induction, or superstardom" – protégés studied under mentors who published research for which they were conferred a prize after the protégés' mentorship. Studying original topics rather than these mentors' research-topics was also positively associated with success.147148 Highly productive partnerships are also a topic of research – e.g. "super-ties" of frequent co-authorship of two individuals who can complement skills, likely also the result of other factors such as mutual trust, conviction, commitment and fun.149150

Study of successful scientists and processes, general skills and activities

The emergence or origin of ideas by successful scientists is also a topic of research, for example reviewing existing ideas on how Mendel made his discoveries,151 – or more generally, the process of discovery by scientists. Science is a "multifaceted process of appropriation, copying, extending, or combining ideas and inventions" [and other types of knowledge or information], and not an isolated process.152 There are also few studies investigating scientists' habits, common modes of thinking, reading habits, use of information sources, digital literacy skills, and workflows.153154155156157

Labor advantage

A study theorized that in many disciplines, larger scientific productivity or success by elite universities can be explained by their larger pool of available funded laborers.158159 The study found that university prestige was only associated with higher productivity for faculty with group members, not for faculty publishing alone or the group members themselves. This is presented as evidence that the outsize productivity of elite researchers is not from a more rigorous selection of talent by top universities, but from labor advantages accrued through greater access to funding and the attraction of prestige to graduate and postdoctoral researchers.

Ultimate impacts

Success in science (as indicated in tenure review processes) is often measured in terms of metrics like citations, not in terms of the eventual or potential impact on lives and society,160 which awards (see above) sometimes do. Problems with such metrics are roughly outlined elsewhere in this article and include that reviews replace citations to primary studies.161 There are also proposals for changes to the academic incentives systems that increase the recognition of societal impact in the research process.162

Progress studies

A proposed field of "Progress Studies" could investigate how scientists (or funders or evaluators of scientists) should be acting, "figuring out interventions" and study progress itself.163 The field was explicitly proposed in a 2019 essay and described as an applied science that prescribes action.164

As and for acceleration of progress

A study suggests that improving the way science is done could accelerate the rate of scientific discovery and its applications which could be useful for finding urgent solutions to humanity's problems, improve humanity's conditions, and enhance understanding of nature. Metascientific studies can seek to identify aspects of science that need improvement, and develop ways to improve them.165 If science is accepted as the fundamental engine of economic growth and social progress, this could raise "the question of what we – as a society – can do to accelerate science, and to direct science toward solving society's most important problems."166 However, one of the authors clarified that a one-size-fits-all approach is not thought to be right answer – for example, in funding, DARPA models, curiosity-driven methods, allowing "a single reviewer to champion a project even if his or her peers do not agree", and various other approaches all have their uses. Nevertheless, evaluation of them can help build knowledge of what works or works best.167

Reforms

Meta-research identifying flaws in scientific practice has inspired reforms in science. These reforms seek to address and fix problems in scientific practice which lead to low-quality or inefficient research.

A 2015 study lists "fragmented" efforts in meta-research.168

Pre-registration

Further information: Pre-registration (science) and Clinical trial registration

The practice of registering a scientific study before it is conducted is called pre-registration. It arose as a means to address the replication crisis. Pregistration requires the submission of a registered report, which is then accepted for publication or rejected by a journal based on theoretical justification, experimental design, and the proposed statistical analysis. Pre-registration of studies serves to prevent publication bias (e.g. not publishing negative results), reduce data dredging, and increase replicability.169170

Reporting standards

Further information: CONSORT and EQUATOR Network

Studies showing poor consistency and quality of reporting have demonstrated the need for reporting standards and guidelines in science, which has led to the rise of organisations that produce such standards, such as CONSORT (Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials) and the EQUATOR Network.

The EQUATOR (Enhancing the QUAlity and Transparency Of health Research)171 Network is an international initiative aimed at promoting transparent and accurate reporting of health research studies to enhance the value and reliability of medical research literature.172 The EQUATOR Network was established with the goals of raising awareness of the importance of good reporting of research, assisting in the development, dissemination and implementation of reporting guidelines for different types of study designs, monitoring the status of the quality of reporting of research studies in the health sciences literature, and conducting research relating to issues that impact the quality of reporting of health research studies.173 The Network acts as an "umbrella" organisation, bringing together developers of reporting guidelines, medical journal editors and peer reviewers, research funding bodies, and other key stakeholders with a mutual interest in improving the quality of research publications and research itself.

Applications

Information and communications technologies

See also: Group decision-making, List of academic databases and search engines, and Scientific communication

Metascience is used in the creation and improvement of technical systems (ICTs) and standards of science evaluation, incentivation, communication, commissioning, funding, regulation, production, management, use and publication. Such can be called "applied metascience"174[better source needed] and may seek to explore ways to increase quantity, quality and positive impact of research. One example for such is the development of alternative metrics.175

Study screening and feedback

Various websites or tools also identify inappropriate studies and/or enable feedback such as PubPeer, Cochrane's Risk of Bias Tool176 and RetractionWatch. Medical and academic disputes are as ancient as antiquity and a study calls for research into "constructive and obsessive criticism" and into policies to "help strengthen social media into a vibrant forum for discussion, and not merely an arena for gladiator matches".177 Feedback to studies can be found via altmetrics which is often integrated at the website of the study – most often as an embedded Altmetrics badge – but may often be incomplete, such as only showing social media discussions that link to the study directly but not those that link to news reports about the study. (see above)

Tools used, modified, extended or investigated

Tools may get developed with metaresearch or can be used or investigated by such. Notable examples may include:

  • Search engines like Google Scholar are used to find studies and the notification service Google Alerts enables notifications for new studies matching specified search terms. Scholarly communication infrastructure includes search databases.178
  • Shadow library Sci-hub is a topic of metascience179
  • Personal knowledge management systems for research-, knowledge- and task management, such as saving information in organized ways180 with multi-document text editors for future use181182 Such systems could be described as part of, along with e.g. Web browser (tabs-addons183 etc) and search software,[additional citation(s) needed] "mind-machine partnerships" that could be investigated by metascience for how they could improve science.184
  • Scholia – efforts to open scholarly publication metadata and use it via Wikidata.185 (see above)
  • Various software enables common metascientific practices such as bibliometric analysis.186

Development

According to a study "a simple way to check how often studies have been repeated, and whether or not the original findings are confirmed" is needed due to reproducibility issues in science.187188 A study suggests a tool for screening studies for early warning signs for research fraud.189

Medicine

See also: Profit motive § Criticisms

Clinical research in medicine is often of low quality, and many studies cannot be replicated.190191 An estimated 85% of research funding is wasted.192 Additionally, the presence of bias affects research quality.193 The pharmaceutical industry exerts substantial influence on the design and execution of medical research. Conflicts of interest are common among authors of medical literature194 and among editors of medical journals. While almost all medical journals require their authors to disclose conflicts of interest, editors are not required to do so.195 Financial conflicts of interest have been linked to higher rates of positive study results. In antidepressant trials, pharmaceutical sponsorship is the best predictor of trial outcome.196

Blinding is another focus of meta-research, as error caused by poor blinding is a source of experimental bias. Blinding is not well reported in medical literature, and widespread misunderstanding of the subject has resulted in poor implementation of blinding in clinical trials.197 Furthermore, failure of blinding is rarely measured or reported.198 Research showing the failure of blinding in antidepressant trials has led some scientists to argue that antidepressants are no better than placebo.199200 In light of meta-research showing failures of blinding, CONSORT standards recommend that all clinical trials assess and report the quality of blinding.201

Studies have shown that systematic reviews of existing research evidence are sub-optimally used in planning a new research or summarizing the results.202 Cumulative meta-analyses of studies evaluating the effectiveness of medical interventions have shown that many clinical trials could have been avoided if a systematic review of existing evidence was done prior to conducting a new trial.203204205 For example, Lau et al.206 analyzed 33 clinical trials (involving 36974 patients) evaluating the effectiveness of intravenous streptokinase for acute myocardial infarction. Their cumulative meta-analysis demonstrated that 25 of 33 trials could have been avoided if a systematic review was conducted prior to conducting a new trial. In other words, randomizing 34542 patients was potentially unnecessary. One study207 analyzed 1523 clinical trials included in 227 meta-analyses and concluded that "less than one quarter of relevant prior studies" were cited. They also confirmed earlier findings that most clinical trial reports do not present systematic review to justify the research or summarize the results.208

Many treatments used in modern medicine have been proven to be ineffective, or even harmful. A 2007 study by John Ioannidis found that it took an average of ten years for the medical community to stop referencing popular practices after their efficacy was unequivocally disproven.209210

Psychology

Further information: Replication crisis

Metascience has revealed significant problems in psychological research. The field suffers from high bias, low reproducibility, and widespread misuse of statistics.211212213 The replication crisis affects psychology more strongly than any other field; as many as two-thirds of highly publicized findings may be impossible to replicate.214 Meta-research finds that 80-95% of psychological studies support their initial hypotheses, which strongly implies the existence of publication bias.215

The replication crisis has led to renewed efforts to re-test important findings.216217 In response to concerns about publication bias and p-hacking, more than 140 psychology journals have adopted result-blind peer review, in which studies are pre-registered and published without regard for their outcome.218 An analysis of these reforms estimated that 61 percent of result-blind studies produce null results, in contrast with 5 to 20 percent in earlier research. This analysis shows that result-blind peer review substantially reduces publication bias.219

Psychologists routinely confuse statistical significance with practical importance, enthusiastically reporting great certainty in unimportant facts.220 Some psychologists have responded with an increased use of effect size statistics, rather than sole reliance on the p values.

Physics

Richard Feynman noted that estimates of physical constants were closer to published values than would be expected by chance. This was believed to be the result of confirmation bias: results that agreed with existing literature were more likely to be believed, and therefore published. Physicists now implement blinding to prevent this kind of bias.221

Computer Science

Web measurement studies are essential for understanding the workings of the modern Web, particularly in the fields of security and privacy. However, these studies often require custom-built or modified crawling setups, leading to a plethora of analysis tools for similar tasks. In a paper by Nurullah Demir et al., the authors surveyed 117 recent research papers to derive best practices for Web-based measurement studies and establish criteria for reproducibility and replicability. They found that experimental setups and other critical information for reproducing and replicating results are often missing. In a large-scale Web measurement study on 4.5 million pages with 24 different measurement setups, the authors demonstrated the impact of slight differences in experimental setups on the overall results, emphasizing the need for accurate and comprehensive documentation.222

Organizations and institutes

Further information: List of metascience research centers

There are several organizations and universities across the globe which work on meta-research – these include the Meta-Research Innovation Center at Berlin,223 the Meta-Research Innovation Center at Stanford,224225 the Meta-Research Center at Tilburg University, the Meta-research & Evidence Synthesis Unit, The George Institute for Global Health at India and Center for Open Science. Organizations that develop tools for metascience include OurResearch, Center for Scientific Integrity and altmetrics companies. There is an annual Metascience Conference hosted by the Association for Interdisciplinary Meta-Research and Open Science (AIMOS) and biannual conference hosted by the Centre for Open Science.226227

See also

Further reading

Journals

Conferences

References

  1. Ioannidis, John P. A.; Fanelli, Daniele; Dunne, Debbie Drake; Goodman, Steven N. (2 October 2015). "Meta-research: Evaluation and Improvement of Research Methods and Practices". PLOS Biology. 13 (10): e1002264. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002264. ISSN 1544-9173. PMC 4592065. PMID 26431313. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4592065

  2. Bach, Becky (8 December 2015). "On communicating science and uncertainty: A podcast with John Ioannidis". Scope. Retrieved 20 May 2019. https://scopeblog.stanford.edu/2015/12/08/on-communicating-science-and-uncertainty-a-podcast-with-john-ioannidis/

  3. Schor, Stanley; Karten, Irving (March 28, 1966). "Statistical Evaluation of Medical Journal Manuscripts". Journal of the American Medical Association. 195 (13): 1123–1128. doi:10.1001/jama.1966.03100130097026. PMID 5952081. https://jamanetwork.com/journals/jama/article-abstract/658800

  4. Pashler, Harold; Harris, Christine R. (2012). "Is the Replicability Crisis Overblown? Three Arguments Examined". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 7 (6): 531–536. doi:10.1177/1745691612463401. ISSN 1745-6916. PMID 26168109. S2CID 1342421. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1745691612463401

  5. Nishikawa-Pacher, Andreas; Heck, Tamara; Schoch, Kerstin (4 October 2022). "Open Editors: A dataset of scholarly journals' editorial board positions". Research Evaluation. 32 (2): 228–243. doi:10.1093/reseval/rvac037. eISSN 1471-5449. ISSN 0958-2029. https://doi.org/10.1093%2Freseval%2Frvac037

  6. Schor, Stanley (1966). "Statistical Evaluation of Medical Journal Manuscripts". JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association. 195 (13): 1123–1128. doi:10.1001/jama.1966.03100130097026. ISSN 0098-7484. PMID 5952081. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  7. Comroe, Julius; Comroe, Robert (1976). "Scientific Basis for the Support of Biomedical Science". Science. 192 (4235): 105–11. Bibcode:1976Sci...192..105C. doi:10.1126/science.769161. JSTOR 1741888. PMID 769161. Retrieved 2023-12-14. https://www.jstor.org/stable/1741888

  8. Ioannidis, JP (August 2005). "Why most published research findings are false". PLOS Medicine. 2 (8): e124. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124. PMC 1182327. PMID 16060722. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1182327

  9. "Highly Cited Researchers". Retrieved September 17, 2015. http://highlycited.com/

  10. Medicine - Stanford Prevention Research Center. John P.A. Ioannidis https://profiles.stanford.edu/john-ioannidis

  11. Robert Lee Hotz (September 14, 2007). "Most Science Studies Appear to Be Tainted By Sloppy Analysis". Wall Street Journal. Dow Jones & Company. Retrieved 2016-12-05. /w/index.php?title=Robert_Lee_Hotz&action=edit&redlink=1

  12. Howick J, Koletsi D, Pandis N, Fleming PS, Loef M, Walach H, Schmidt S, Ioannidis JA. The quality of evidence for medical interventions does not improve or worsen: a metaepidemiological study of Cochrane reviews. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 2020;126:154–159 [1] https://www.jclinepi.com/article/S0895-4356(20)30777-0/fulltext

  13. "Researching the researchers". Nature Genetics. 46 (5): 417. 2014. doi:10.1038/ng.2972. ISSN 1061-4036. PMID 24769715. https://doi.org/10.1038%2Fng.2972

  14. Enserink, Martin (2018). "Research on research". Science. 361 (6408): 1178–1179. Bibcode:2018Sci...361.1178E. doi:10.1126/science.361.6408.1178. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 30237336. S2CID 206626417. /wiki/Bibcode_(identifier)

  15. Rennie, Drummond (1990). "Editorial Peer Review in Biomedical Publication". JAMA. 263 (10): 1317–1441. doi:10.1001/jama.1990.03440100011001. ISSN 0098-7484. PMID 2304208. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  16. Harriman, Stephanie L.; Kowalczuk, Maria K.; Simera, Iveta; Wager, Elizabeth (2016). "A new forum for research on research integrity and peer review". Research Integrity and Peer Review. 1 (1): 5. doi:10.1186/s41073-016-0010-y. ISSN 2058-8615. PMC 5794038. PMID 29451544. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5794038

  17. Ioannidis, John P. A.; Fanelli, Daniele; Dunne, Debbie Drake; Goodman, Steven N. (2 October 2015). "Meta-research: Evaluation and Improvement of Research Methods and Practices". PLOS Biology. 13 (10): e1002264. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002264. ISSN 1544-9173. PMC 4592065. PMID 26431313. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4592065

  18. Ioannidis, John P. A.; Fanelli, Daniele; Dunne, Debbie Drake; Goodman, Steven N. (2 October 2015). "Meta-research: Evaluation and Improvement of Research Methods and Practices". PLOS Biology. 13 (10): e1002264. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002264. ISSN 1544-9173. PMC 4592065. PMID 26431313. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4592065

  19. Fanelli, Daniele; Costas, Rodrigo; Ioannidis, John P. A. (2017). "Meta-assessment of bias in science". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 114 (14): 3714–3719. Bibcode:2017PNAS..114.3714F. doi:10.1073/pnas.1618569114. ISSN 1091-6490. PMC 5389310. PMID 28320937. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5389310

  20. Check Hayden, Erika (2013). "Weak statistical standards implicated in scientific irreproducibility". Nature. doi:10.1038/nature.2013.14131. S2CID 211729036. Retrieved 9 May 2019. https://www.nature.com/news/weak-statistical-standards-implicated-in-scientific-irreproducibility-1.14131

  21. Krauss, A (2024). Science of Science: Understanding the Foundations and Limits of Science from an Interdisciplinary Perspective. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/9780198937401.001.0001. ISBN 978-0-19-893740-1. 978-0-19-893740-1

  22. Markowitz, David M.; Hancock, Jeffrey T. (2016). "Linguistic obfuscation in fraudulent science". Journal of Language and Social Psychology. 35 (4): 435–445. doi:10.1177/0261927X15614605. S2CID 146174471. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  23. Ding, Y. (2010). "Applying weighted PageRank to author citation networks". Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology. 62 (2): 236–245. arXiv:1102.1760. doi:10.1002/asi.21452. S2CID 3752804. /wiki/ArXiv_(identifier)

  24. Galipeau, James; Moher, David; Campbell, Craig; Hendry, Paul; Cameron, D. William; Palepu, Anita; Hébert, Paul C. (March 2015). "A systematic review highlights a knowledge gap regarding the effectiveness of health-related training programs in journalology". Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 68 (3): 257–265. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2014.09.024. PMID 25510373. https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.jclinepi.2014.09.024

  25. Wilson, Mitch; Moher, David (March 2019). "The Changing Landscape of Journalology in Medicine". Seminars in Nuclear Medicine. 49 (2): 105–114. doi:10.1053/j.semnuclmed.2018.11.009. hdl:10393/38493. PMID 30819390. S2CID 73471103. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  26. Couzin-Frankel, Jennifer (18 September 2018). "'Journalologists' use scientific methods to study academic publishing. Is their work improving science?". Science. doi:10.1126/science.aav4758. S2CID 115360831. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  27. Couzin-Frankel, Jennifer (18 September 2018). "'Journalologists' use scientific methods to study academic publishing. Is their work improving science?". Science. doi:10.1126/science.aav4758. S2CID 115360831. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  28. Couzin-Frankel, Jennifer (18 September 2018). "'Journalologists' use scientific methods to study academic publishing. Is their work improving science?". Science. doi:10.1126/science.aav4758. S2CID 115360831. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  29. Ioannidis, John P. A.; Fanelli, Daniele; Dunne, Debbie Drake; Goodman, Steven N. (2 October 2015). "Meta-research: Evaluation and Improvement of Research Methods and Practices". PLOS Biology. 13 (10): e1002264. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002264. ISSN 1544-9173. PMC 4592065. PMID 26431313. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4592065

  30. Schooler, J. W. (2014). "Metascience could rescue the 'replication crisis'". Nature. 515 (7525): 9. Bibcode:2014Natur.515....9S. doi:10.1038/515009a. PMID 25373639. https://doi.org/10.1038%2F515009a

  31. Smith, Noah (2 November 2017). "Why 'Statistical Significance' Is Often Insignificant". Bloomberg.com. Retrieved 7 November 2017. https://www.bloomberg.com/view/articles/2017-11-02/why-statistical-significance-is-often-insignificant

  32. Pashler, Harold; Wagenmakers, Eric Jan (2012). "Editors' Introduction to the Special Section on Replicability in Psychological Science: A Crisis of Confidence?". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 7 (6): 528–530. doi:10.1177/1745691612465253. PMID 26168108. S2CID 26361121. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  33. Ioannidis, John P. A.; Fanelli, Daniele; Dunne, Debbie Drake; Goodman, Steven N. (2 October 2015). "Meta-research: Evaluation and Improvement of Research Methods and Practices". PLOS Biology. 13 (10): e1002264. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002264. ISSN 1544-9173. PMC 4592065. PMID 26431313. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4592065

  34. Gary Marcus (May 1, 2013). "The Crisis in Social Psychology That Isn't". The New Yorker. http://www.newyorker.com/tech/elements/the-crisis-in-social-psychology-that-isnt

  35. Jonah Lehrer (December 13, 2010). "The Truth Wears Off". The New Yorker. http://www.newyorker.com/magazine/2010/12/13/the-truth-wears-off

  36. "Dozens of major cancer studies can't be replicated". Science News. 7 December 2021. Retrieved 19 January 2022. https://www.sciencenews.org/article/cancer-biology-studies-research-replication-reproducibility

  37. "Reproducibility Project: Cancer Biology". www.cos.io. Center for Open Science. Retrieved 19 January 2022. https://www.cos.io/rpcb

  38. Staddon, John (2017) Scientific Method: How science works, fails to work or pretends to work. Taylor and Francis.

  39. Yeung, Andy W. K. (2017). "Do Neuroscience Journals Accept Replications? A Survey of Literature". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 11: 468. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2017.00468. ISSN 1662-5161. PMC 5611708. PMID 28979201. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5611708

  40. Martin, G. N.; Clarke, Richard M. (2017). "Are Psychology Journals Anti-replication? A Snapshot of Editorial Practices". Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 523. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00523. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 5387793. PMID 28443044. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5387793

  41. Ioannidis, John P. A.; Fanelli, Daniele; Dunne, Debbie Drake; Goodman, Steven N. (2 October 2015). "Meta-research: Evaluation and Improvement of Research Methods and Practices". PLOS Biology. 13 (10): e1002264. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002264. ISSN 1544-9173. PMC 4592065. PMID 26431313. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4592065

  42. Ioannidis, John P. A.; Fanelli, Daniele; Dunne, Debbie Drake; Goodman, Steven N. (2 October 2015). "Meta-research: Evaluation and Improvement of Research Methods and Practices". PLOS Biology. 13 (10): e1002264. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002264. ISSN 1544-9173. PMC 4592065. PMID 26431313. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4592065

  43. Binswanger, Mathias (2015). "How Nonsense Became Excellence: Forcing Professors to Publish". In Welpe, Isabell M.; Wollersheim, Jutta; Ringelhan, Stefanie; Osterloh, Margit (eds.). Incentives and Performance. Springer International Publishing. pp. 19–32. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-09785-5_2. ISBN 978-3319097855. S2CID 110698382. {{cite book}}: |work= ignored (help) 978-3319097855

  44. Edwards, Marc A.; Roy, Siddhartha (2016-09-22). "Academic Research in the 21st Century: Maintaining Scientific Integrity in a Climate of Perverse Incentives and Hypercompetition". Environmental Engineering Science. 34 (1): 51–61. doi:10.1089/ees.2016.0223. PMC 5206685. PMID 28115824. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5206685

  45. Brookshire, Bethany (21 October 2016). "Blame bad incentives for bad science". Science News. Retrieved 11 July 2019. https://www.sciencenews.org/blog/scicurious/blame-bad-incentives-bad-science

  46. Smaldino, Paul E.; McElreath, Richard (2016). "The natural selection of bad science". Royal Society Open Science. 3 (9): 160384. arXiv:1605.09511. Bibcode:2016RSOS....360384S. doi:10.1098/rsos.160384. PMC 5043322. PMID 27703703. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5043322

  47. Chapman, Colin A.; Bicca-Marques, Júlio César; Calvignac-Spencer, Sébastien; Fan, Pengfei; Fashing, Peter J.; Gogarten, Jan; Guo, Songtao; Hemingway, Claire A.; Leendertz, Fabian; Li, Baoguo; Matsuda, Ikki; Hou, Rong; Serio-Silva, Juan Carlos; Chr. Stenseth, Nils (4 December 2019). "Games academics play and their consequences: how authorship, h -index and journal impact factors are shaping the future of academia". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 286 (1916): 20192047. doi:10.1098/rspb.2019.2047. ISSN 0962-8452. PMC 6939250. PMID 31797732. S2CID 208605640. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6939250

  48. Holcombe, Alex O. (September 2019). "Contributorship, Not Authorship: Use CRediT to Indicate Who Did What". Publications. 7 (3): 48. doi:10.3390/publications7030048. https://doi.org/10.3390%2Fpublications7030048

  49. McNutt, Marcia K.; Bradford, Monica; Drazen, Jeffrey M.; Hanson, Brooks; Howard, Bob; Jamieson, Kathleen Hall; Kiermer, Véronique; Marcus, Emilie; Pope, Barbara Kline; Schekman, Randy; Swaminathan, Sowmya; Stang, Peter J.; Verma, Inder M. (13 March 2018). "Transparency in authors' contributions and responsibilities to promote integrity in scientific publication". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 115 (11): 2557–2560. Bibcode:2018PNAS..115.2557M. doi:10.1073/pnas.1715374115. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 5856527. PMID 29487213. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5856527

  50. Brand, Amy; Allen, Liz; Altman, Micah; Hlava, Marjorie; Scott, Jo (1 April 2015). "Beyond authorship: attribution, contribution, collaboration, and credit". Learned Publishing. 28 (2): 151–155. doi:10.1087/20150211. S2CID 45167271. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/274098676

  51. Singh Chawla, Dalmeet (October 2015). "Digital badges aim to clear up politics of authorship". Nature. 526 (7571): 145–146. Bibcode:2015Natur.526..145S. doi:10.1038/526145a. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 26432249. S2CID 256770827. https://doi.org/10.1038%2F526145a

  52. Fire, Michael; Guestrin, Carlos (1 June 2019). "Over-optimization of academic publishing metrics: observing Goodhart's Law in action". GigaScience. 8 (6): giz053. doi:10.1093/gigascience/giz053. PMC 6541803. PMID 31144712. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6541803

  53. Elson, Malte; Huff, Markus; Utz, Sonja (1 March 2020). "Metascience on Peer Review: Testing the Effects of a Study's Originality and Statistical Significance in a Field Experiment". Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science. 3 (1): 53–65. doi:10.1177/2515245919895419. ISSN 2515-2459. S2CID 212778011. https://psyarxiv.com/gyds8/

  54. McLean, Robert K D; Sen, Kunal (1 April 2019). "Making a difference in the real world? A meta-analysis of the quality of use-oriented research using the Research Quality Plus approach". Research Evaluation. 28 (2): 123–135. doi:10.1093/reseval/rvy026. https://doi.org/10.1093%2Freseval%2Frvy026

  55. "Bringing Rigor to Relevant Questions: How Social Science Research Can Improve Youth Outcomes in the Real World" (PDF). Retrieved 22 November 2021. http://wtgrantfoundation.org/library/uploads/2017/05/How-Social-Science-Research-Can-Improve-Youth-Outcomes_WTG2017.pdf

  56. Fecher, Benedikt; Friesike, Sascha; Hebing, Marcel; Linek, Stephanie (20 June 2017). "A reputation economy: how individual reward considerations trump systemic arguments for open access to data". Palgrave Communications. 3 (1): 1–10. doi:10.1057/palcomms.2017.51. hdl:11108/308. ISSN 2055-1045. S2CID 34449408. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  57. La Porta, Caterina AM; Zapperi, Stefano (1 December 2022). "America's top universities reap the benefit of Italian-trained scientists". Nature Italy. doi:10.1038/d43978-022-00163-5. S2CID 254331807. Retrieved 18 December 2022. https://www.nature.com/articles/d43978-022-00163-5

  58. Leydesdorff, L. and Milojevic, S., "Scientometrics" arXiv:1208.4566 (2013), forthcoming in: Lynch, M. (editor), International Encyclopedia of Social and Behavioral Sciences subsection 85030. (2015) /wiki/Loet_Leydesdorff

  59. Fire, Michael; Guestrin, Carlos (1 June 2019). "Over-optimization of academic publishing metrics: observing Goodhart's Law in action". GigaScience. 8 (6): giz053. doi:10.1093/gigascience/giz053. PMC 6541803. PMID 31144712. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6541803

  60. Krauss, A (2024). Science of Science: Understanding the Foundations and Limits of Science from an Interdisciplinary Perspective. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/9780198937401.001.0001. ISBN 978-0-19-893740-1. 978-0-19-893740-1

  61. Singh, Navinder (8 October 2021). "Plea to publish less". arXiv:2201.07985 [physics.soc-ph]. /wiki/ArXiv_(identifier)

  62. Manchanda, Saurav; Karypis, George (November 2021). "Evaluating Scholarly Impact: Towards Content-Aware Bibliometrics". Proceedings of the 2021 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing. Association for Computational Linguistics. pp. 6041–6053. doi:10.18653/v1/2021.emnlp-main.488. S2CID 243865632. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  63. Manchanda, Saurav; Karypis, George. "Importance Assessment in Scholarly Networks" (PDF). http://ceur-ws.org/Vol-2831/paper17.pdf

  64. Nielsen, Kristian H. (1 March 2021). "Science and public policy". Metascience. 30 (1): 79–81. doi:10.1007/s11016-020-00581-5. ISSN 1467-9981. PMC 7605730. S2CID 226237994. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7605730

  65. Bostrom, Nick (2014). Superintelligence: Paths, Dangers, Strategies. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 229–237. ISBN 978-0199678112. 978-0199678112

  66. Ord, Toby (2020). The Precipice: Existential Risk and the Future of Humanity. United Kingdom: Bloomsbury Publishing. p. 200. ISBN 978-1526600219. 978-1526600219

  67. "Technology is changing faster than regulators can keep up - here's how to close the gap". World Economic Forum. 21 June 2018. Retrieved 27 January 2022. https://www.weforum.org/agenda/2018/06/law-too-slow-for-new-tech-how-keep-up/

  68. "Nobel prize-winning work is concentrated in minority of scientific fields". phys.org. Retrieved 17 August 2020. https://phys.org/news/2020-07-nobel-prize-winning-minority-scientific-fields.html

  69. Ioannidis, John P. A.; Cristea, Ioana-Alina; Boyack, Kevin W. (29 July 2020). "Work honored by Nobel prizes clusters heavily in a few scientific fields". PLOS ONE. 15 (7): e0234612. Bibcode:2020PLoSO..1534612I. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0234612. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 7390258. PMID 32726312. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7390258

  70. Nielsen, Kristian H. (1 March 2021). "Science and public policy". Metascience. 30 (1): 79–81. doi:10.1007/s11016-020-00581-5. ISSN 1467-9981. PMC 7605730. S2CID 226237994. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7605730

  71. Fortunato, Santo; Bergstrom, Carl T.; Börner, Katy; Evans, James A.; Helbing, Dirk; Milojević, Staša; Petersen, Alexander M.; Radicchi, Filippo; Sinatra, Roberta; Uzzi, Brian; Vespignani, Alessandro; Waltman, Ludo; Wang, Dashun; Barabási, Albert-László (2 March 2018). "Science of science". Science. 359 (6379): eaao0185. doi:10.1126/science.aao0185. PMC 5949209. PMID 29496846. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323502497

  72. Elson, Malte; Huff, Markus; Utz, Sonja (1 March 2020). "Metascience on Peer Review: Testing the Effects of a Study's Originality and Statistical Significance in a Field Experiment". Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science. 3 (1): 53–65. doi:10.1177/2515245919895419. ISSN 2515-2459. S2CID 212778011. https://psyarxiv.com/gyds8/

  73. Fire, Michael; Guestrin, Carlos (1 June 2019). "Over-optimization of academic publishing metrics: observing Goodhart's Law in action". GigaScience. 8 (6): giz053. doi:10.1093/gigascience/giz053. PMC 6541803. PMID 31144712. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6541803

  74. Fajardo-Ortiz, David; Hornbostel, Stefan; Montenegro de Wit, Maywa; Shattuck, Annie (22 June 2022). "Funding CRISPR: Understanding the role of government and philanthropic institutions in supporting academic research within the CRISPR innovation system". Quantitative Science Studies. 3 (2): 443–456. arXiv:2009.11920. doi:10.1162/qss_a_00187. S2CID 235266330. https://doi.org/10.1162%2Fqss_a_00187

  75. "Science as a Global Public Good". International Science Council. 8 October 2021. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://council.science/current/news/science-as-a-global-public-good/

  76. Jamieson, Kathleen Hall; Kahan, Dan; Scheufele, Dietram A. (17 May 2017). The Oxford Handbook of the Science of Science Communication. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0190497637. 978-0190497637

  77. Grochala, Rafał (16 December 2019). "Science communication in online media: influence of press releases on coverage of genetics and CRISPR". bioRxiv. doi:10.1101/2019.12.13.875278. S2CID 213125031. https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2019.12.13.875278v1.abstract

  78. G. Manickam Govindaraju; Kalei Joethi Sahadevan; Tan Poh Ling (February 2019). "Framing Analysis of News Coverage on Renewable Energy in the Star Online News Portal" (PDF). Journal of Engineering Science and Technology. Retrieved 22 November 2021. http://jestec.taylors.edu.my/Special%20Issue%20on%20SU18/SU18_04.pdf

  79. MacLaughlin, Ansel; Wihbey, John; Smith, David (15 June 2018). "Predicting News Coverage of Scientific Articles". Proceedings of the International AAAI Conference on Web and Social Media. 12 (1). doi:10.1609/icwsm.v12i1.14999. ISSN 2334-0770. S2CID 49412893. https://ojs.aaai.org/index.php/ICWSM/article/view/14999

  80. Carrigan, Mark; Jordan, Katy (4 November 2021). "Platforms and Institutions in the Post-Pandemic University: a Case Study of Social Media and the Impact Agenda". Postdigital Science and Education. 4 (2): 354–372. doi:10.1007/s42438-021-00269-x. ISSN 2524-4868. S2CID 243760357. https://doi.org/10.1007%2Fs42438-021-00269-x

  81. Fortunato, Santo; Bergstrom, Carl T.; Börner, Katy; Evans, James A.; Helbing, Dirk; Milojević, Staša; Petersen, Alexander M.; Radicchi, Filippo; Sinatra, Roberta; Uzzi, Brian; Vespignani, Alessandro; Waltman, Ludo; Wang, Dashun; Barabási, Albert-László (2 March 2018). "Science of science". Science. 359 (6379): eaao0185. doi:10.1126/science.aao0185. PMC 5949209. PMID 29496846. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323502497

  82. Baykoucheva, Svetla (2015). "Measuring attention". Managing Scientific Information and Research Data: 127–136. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-100195-0.00014-7. ISBN 978-0081001950. 978-0081001950

  83. Zagorova, Olga; Ulloa, Roberto; Weller, Katrin; Flöck, Fabian (12 April 2022). ""I updated the ": The evolution of references in the English Wikipedia and the implications for altmetrics". Quantitative Science Studies. 3 (1): 147–173. doi:10.1162/qss_a_00171. S2CID 222177064. https://doi.org/10.1162%2Fqss_a_00171

  84. Zagorova, Olga; Ulloa, Roberto; Weller, Katrin; Flöck, Fabian (12 April 2022). ""I updated the ": The evolution of references in the English Wikipedia and the implications for altmetrics". Quantitative Science Studies. 3 (1): 147–173. doi:10.1162/qss_a_00171. S2CID 222177064. https://doi.org/10.1162%2Fqss_a_00171

  85. Williams, Ann E. (12 June 2017). "Altmetrics: an overview and evaluation". Online Information Review. 41 (3): 311–317. doi:10.1108/OIR-10-2016-0294. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  86. Fortunato, Santo; Bergstrom, Carl T.; Börner, Katy; Evans, James A.; Helbing, Dirk; Milojević, Staša; Petersen, Alexander M.; Radicchi, Filippo; Sinatra, Roberta; Uzzi, Brian; Vespignani, Alessandro; Waltman, Ludo; Wang, Dashun; Barabási, Albert-László (2 March 2018). "Science of science". Science. 359 (6379): eaao0185. doi:10.1126/science.aao0185. PMC 5949209. PMID 29496846. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323502497

  87. Gurevitch, Jessica; Koricheva, Julia; Nakagawa, Shinichi; Stewart, Gavin (March 2018). "Meta-analysis and the science of research synthesis". Nature. 555 (7695): 175–182. Bibcode:2018Natur.555..175G. doi:10.1038/nature25753. hdl:1959.4/unsworks_71830. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 29517004. S2CID 3761687. /wiki/Bibcode_(identifier)

  88. Balbi, Stefano; Bagstad, Kenneth J.; Magrach, Ainhoa; Sanz, Maria Jose; Aguilar-Amuchastegui, Naikoa; Giupponi, Carlo; Villa, Ferdinando (17 February 2022). "The global environmental agenda urgently needs a semantic web of knowledge". Environmental Evidence. 11 (1): 5. Bibcode:2022EnvEv..11....5B. doi:10.1186/s13750-022-00258-y. hdl:10278/5023700. ISSN 2047-2382. PMC 11378787. PMID 39294723. S2CID 246872765. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11378787

  89. Khalil, Mohammed M. (2016). "Improving Science for a Better Future". How Should Humanity Steer the Future?. The Frontiers Collection. Springer International Publishing. pp. 113–126. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-20717-9_11. ISBN 978-3-319-20716-2. 978-3-319-20716-2

  90. Zagorova, Olga; Ulloa, Roberto; Weller, Katrin; Flöck, Fabian (12 April 2022). ""I updated the ": The evolution of references in the English Wikipedia and the implications for altmetrics". Quantitative Science Studies. 3 (1): 147–173. doi:10.1162/qss_a_00171. S2CID 222177064. https://doi.org/10.1162%2Fqss_a_00171

  91. Bottesini, Julia G.; Aschwanden, Christie; Rhemtulla, Mijke; Vazire, Simine. "How Do Science Journalists Evaluate Psychology Research?". psyarxiv.com. https://psyarxiv.com/26kr3/

  92. Dunlop, Lynda; Veneu, Fernanda (1 September 2019). "Controversies in Science". Science & Education. 28 (6): 689–710. doi:10.1007/s11191-019-00048-y. ISSN 1573-1901. S2CID 255016078. https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/146183/1/LD_final_edits.docx

  93. Norsen, Travis (2016). "Back to the Future: Crowdsourcing Innovation by Refocusing Science Education". How Should Humanity Steer the Future?. The Frontiers Collection. pp. 85–95. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-20717-9_9. ISBN 978-3-319-20716-2. 978-3-319-20716-2

  94. Bschir, Karim (July 2021). "How to make sense of science: Mano Singham: The great paradox of science: why its conclusions can be relied upon even though they cannot be proven. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2019, 332 pp, £ 22.99 HB". Metascience. 30 (2): 327–330. doi:10.1007/s11016-021-00654-z. S2CID 254792908. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  95. "Correcting misconceptions - Understanding Science". 21 April 2022. Retrieved 25 January 2023. https://undsci.berkeley.edu/for-educators/prepare-and-plan/correcting-misconceptions/

  96. Philipp-Muller, Aviva; Lee, Spike W. S.; Petty, Richard E. (26 July 2022). "Why are people antiscience, and what can we do about it?". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 119 (30): e2120755119. Bibcode:2022PNAS..11920755P. doi:10.1073/pnas.2120755119. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 9335320. PMID 35858405. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9335320

  97. "The 4 bases of anti-science beliefs – and what to do about them". SCIENMAG: Latest Science and Health News. 11 July 2022. Retrieved 25 January 2023. https://scienmag.com/the-4-bases-of-anti-science-beliefs-and-what-to-do-about-them/

  98. Hotez, Peter J. "The Antiscience Movement Is Escalating, Going Global and Killing Thousands". Scientific American. Retrieved 25 January 2023. https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/the-antiscience-movement-is-escalating-going-global-and-killing-thousands/

  99. Fortunato, Santo; Bergstrom, Carl T.; Börner, Katy; Evans, James A.; Helbing, Dirk; Milojević, Staša; Petersen, Alexander M.; Radicchi, Filippo; Sinatra, Roberta; Uzzi, Brian; Vespignani, Alessandro; Waltman, Ludo; Wang, Dashun; Barabási, Albert-László (2 March 2018). "Science of science". Science. 359 (6379): eaao0185. doi:10.1126/science.aao0185. PMC 5949209. PMID 29496846. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323502497

  100. "Science as a Global Public Good". International Science Council. 8 October 2021. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://council.science/current/news/science-as-a-global-public-good/

  101. "Nature Journals To Charge Authors Hefty Fee To Make Scientific Papers Open Access". IFLScience. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://www.iflscience.com/editors-blog/nature-journals-to-charge-authors-hefty-fee-to-make-scientific-papers-open-access/

  102. "Harvard University says it can't afford journal publishers' prices". The Guardian. 24 April 2012. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://www.theguardian.com/science/2012/apr/24/harvard-university-journal-publishers-prices

  103. Van Noorden, Richard (1 March 2013). "Open access: The true cost of science publishing". Nature. 495 (7442): 426–429. Bibcode:2013Natur.495..426V. doi:10.1038/495426a. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 23538808. S2CID 27021567. https://doi.org/10.1038%2F495426a

  104. Tennant, Jonathan P.; Waldner, François; Jacques, Damien C.; Masuzzo, Paola; Collister, Lauren B.; Hartgerink, Chris. H. J. (21 September 2016). "The academic, economic and societal impacts of Open Access: an evidence-based review". F1000Research. 5: 632. doi:10.12688/f1000research.8460.3. PMC 4837983. PMID 27158456. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4837983

  105. "Paywall: The business of scholarship review – analysis of a scandal". New Scientist. Retrieved 28 January 2023. https://www.newscientist.com/article/2181744-paywall-the-business-of-scholarship-review-analysis-of-a-scandal/

  106. Powell, Kendall (1 February 2016). "Does it take too long to publish research?". Nature. 530 (7589): 148–151. Bibcode:2016Natur.530..148P. doi:10.1038/530148a. PMID 26863966. S2CID 1013588. https://doi.org/10.1038%2F530148a

  107. "Open peer review: bringing transparency, accountability, and inclusivity to the peer review process". Impact of Social Sciences. 13 September 2017. Retrieved 28 January 2023. https://blogs.lse.ac.uk/impactofsocialsciences/2017/09/13/open-peer-review-bringing-transparency-accountability-and-inclusivity-to-the-peer-review-process/

  108. Dattani, Saloni. "The Pandemic Uncovered Ways to Speed Up Science". Wired. Retrieved 28 January 2023. https://www.wired.com/story/covid-19-open-science-public-health-data/

  109. "Speeding up the publication process at PLOS ONE". EveryONE. 13 May 2019. Retrieved 28 January 2023. https://everyone.plos.org/2019/05/13/publication_timings_2019/

  110. Singh Chawla, Dalmeet (24 January 2022). "Massive open index of scholarly papers launches". Nature. doi:10.1038/d41586-022-00138-y. Retrieved 14 February 2022. https://www.nature.com/articles/d41586-022-00138-y

  111. "OpenAlex: The Promising Alternative to Microsoft Academic Graph". Singapore Management University (SMU). Retrieved 14 February 2022. https://library.smu.edu.sg/topics-insights/openalex-promising-alternative-microsoft-academic-graph

  112. "OpenAlex Documentation". Retrieved 18 February 2022. https://docs.openalex.org/

  113. Waagmeester, Andra; Willighagen, Egon L.; Su, Andrew I.; Kutmon, Martina; Gayo, Jose Emilio Labra; Fernández-Álvarez, Daniel; Groom, Quentin; Schaap, Peter J.; Verhagen, Lisa M.; Koehorst, Jasper J. (22 January 2021). "A protocol for adding knowledge to Wikidata: aligning resources on human coronaviruses". BMC Biology. 19 (1): 12. doi:10.1186/s12915-020-00940-y. ISSN 1741-7007. PMC 7820539. PMID 33482803. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7820539

  114. Jin, Ching; Ma, Yifang; Uzzi, Brian (5 October 2021). "Scientific prizes and the extraordinary growth of scientific topics". Nature Communications. 12 (1): 5619. arXiv:2012.09269. Bibcode:2021NatCo..12.5619J. doi:10.1038/s41467-021-25712-2. ISSN 2041-1723. PMC 8492701. PMID 34611161. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8492701

  115. Bornmann, Lutz; Haunschild, Robin; Mutz, Rüdiger (7 October 2021). "Growth rates of modern science: a latent piecewise growth curve approach to model publication numbers from established and new literature databases". Humanities and Social Sciences Communications. 8 (1): 1–15. arXiv:2012.07675. doi:10.1057/s41599-021-00903-w. ISSN 2662-9992. S2CID 229156128. /wiki/ArXiv_(identifier)

  116. Park, Michael; Leahey, Erin; Funk, Russell J. (January 2023). "Papers and patents are becoming less disruptive over time". Nature. 613 (7942): 138–144. Bibcode:2023Natur.613..138P. doi:10.1038/s41586-022-05543-x. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 36600070. S2CID 255466666. https://doi.org/10.1038%2Fs41586-022-05543-x

  117. Thompson, Derek (1 December 2021). "America Is Running on Fumes". The Atlantic. Retrieved 27 January 2023. https://www.theatlantic.com/ideas/archive/2021/12/america-innovation-film-science-business/620858/

  118. Collison, Patrick; Nielsen, Michael (16 November 2018). "Science Is Getting Less Bang for Its Buck". The Atlantic. Retrieved 27 January 2023. https://www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2018/11/diminishing-returns-science/575665/

  119. Thompson, Derek (1 December 2021). "America Is Running on Fumes". The Atlantic. Retrieved 27 January 2023. https://www.theatlantic.com/ideas/archive/2021/12/america-innovation-film-science-business/620858/

  120. Park, Michael; Leahey, Erin; Funk, Russell J. (January 2023). "Papers and patents are becoming less disruptive over time". Nature. 613 (7942): 138–144. Bibcode:2023Natur.613..138P. doi:10.1038/s41586-022-05543-x. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 36600070. S2CID 255466666. https://doi.org/10.1038%2Fs41586-022-05543-x

  121. "How to escape scientific stagnation". The Economist. Retrieved 25 January 2023. https://www.economist.com/finance-and-economics/2022/10/26/how-to-escape-scientific-stagnation

  122. Bhattacharya, Jay; Packalen, Mikko (February 2020). "Stagnation and Scientific Incentives" (PDF). National Bureau of Economic Research. https://www.nber.org/system/files/working_papers/w26752/w26752.pdf

  123. Tejada, Patricia Contreras (13 January 2023). "With fewer disruptive studies, is science becoming an echo chamber?". Advanced Science News. Archived from the original on 15 February 2023. Retrieved 15 February 2023. https://www.advancedsciencenews.com/with-fewer-disruptive-studies-is-science-becoming-an-echo-chamber/

  124. Park, Michael; Leahey, Erin; Funk, Russell J. (January 2023). "Papers and patents are becoming less disruptive over time". Nature. 613 (7942): 138–144. Bibcode:2023Natur.613..138P. doi:10.1038/s41586-022-05543-x. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 36600070. S2CID 255466666. https://doi.org/10.1038%2Fs41586-022-05543-x

  125. Bhattacharya, Jay; Packalen, Mikko (February 2020). "Stagnation and Scientific Incentives" (PDF). National Bureau of Economic Research. https://www.nber.org/system/files/working_papers/w26752/w26752.pdf

  126. Bhattacharya, Jay; Packalen, Mikko (February 2020). "Stagnation and Scientific Incentives" (PDF). National Bureau of Economic Research. https://www.nber.org/system/files/working_papers/w26752/w26752.pdf

  127. Wang, Jian; Veugelers, Reinhilde; Stephan, Paula (1 October 2017). "Bias against novelty in science: A cautionary tale for users of bibliometric indicators". Research Policy. 46 (8): 1416–1436. doi:10.1016/j.respol.2017.06.006. hdl:1887/69133. ISSN 0048-7333. https://lirias.kuleuven.be/handle/123456789/590071

  128. Fortunato, Santo; Bergstrom, Carl T.; Börner, Katy; Evans, James A.; Helbing, Dirk; Milojević, Staša; Petersen, Alexander M.; Radicchi, Filippo; Sinatra, Roberta; Uzzi, Brian; Vespignani, Alessandro; Waltman, Ludo; Wang, Dashun; Barabási, Albert-László (2 March 2018). "Science of science". Science. 359 (6379): eaao0185. doi:10.1126/science.aao0185. PMC 5949209. PMID 29496846. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323502497

  129. Petrovich, Eugenio (2020). "Science mapping". www.isko.org. Retrieved 27 January 2023. https://www.isko.org/cyclo/science_mapping#9

  130. Chen, Chaomei (21 March 2017). "Science Mapping: A Systematic Review of the Literature". Journal of Data and Information Science. 2 (2): 1–40. doi:10.1515/jdis-2017-0006. S2CID 57737772. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/313991204

  131. Gutiérrez-Salcedo, M.; Martínez, M. Ángeles; Moral-Munoz, J. A.; Herrera-Viedma, E.; Cobo, M. J. (1 May 2018). "Some bibliometric procedures for analyzing and evaluating research fields". Applied Intelligence. 48 (5): 1275–1287. doi:10.1007/s10489-017-1105-y. hdl:10481/87679. ISSN 1573-7497. S2CID 254227914. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  132. Navarro, V. (31 March 2008). "Politics and health: a neglected area of research". The European Journal of Public Health. 18 (4): 354–355. doi:10.1093/eurpub/ckn040. PMID 18524802. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  133. Overland, Indra; Sovacool, Benjamin K. (1 April 2020). "The misallocation of climate research funding". Energy Research & Social Science. 62: 101349. Bibcode:2020ERSS...6201349O. doi:10.1016/j.erss.2019.101349. hdl:11250/2647605. ISSN 2214-6296. https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.erss.2019.101349

  134. Overland, Indra; Sovacool, Benjamin K. (1 April 2020). "The misallocation of climate research funding". Energy Research & Social Science. 62: 101349. Bibcode:2020ERSS...6201349O. doi:10.1016/j.erss.2019.101349. hdl:11250/2647605. ISSN 2214-6296. https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.erss.2019.101349

  135. Farley-Ripple, Elizabeth N.; Oliver, Kathryn; Boaz, Annette (7 September 2020). "Mapping the community: use of research evidence in policy and practice". Humanities and Social Sciences Communications. 7 (1): 1–10. doi:10.1057/s41599-020-00571-2. ISSN 2662-9992. https://doi.org/10.1057%2Fs41599-020-00571-2

  136. Lamers, Wout S; Boyack, Kevin; Larivière, Vincent; Sugimoto, Cassidy R; van Eck, Nees Jan; Waltman, Ludo; Murray, Dakota (24 December 2021). "Investigating disagreement in the scientific literature". eLife. 10: e72737. doi:10.7554/eLife.72737. ISSN 2050-084X. PMC 8709576. PMID 34951588. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8709576

  137. Stone, Dianna L.; Rosopa, Patrick J. (1 March 2017). "The Advantages and Limitations of Using Meta-analysis in Human Resource Management Research". Human Resource Management Review. 27 (1): 1–7. doi:10.1016/j.hrmr.2016.09.001. ISSN 1053-4822. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  138. Waagmeester, Andra; Willighagen, Egon L.; Su, Andrew I.; Kutmon, Martina; Gayo, Jose Emilio Labra; Fernández-Álvarez, Daniel; Groom, Quentin; Schaap, Peter J.; Verhagen, Lisa M.; Koehorst, Jasper J. (22 January 2021). "A protocol for adding knowledge to Wikidata: aligning resources on human coronaviruses". BMC Biology. 19 (1): 12. doi:10.1186/s12915-020-00940-y. ISSN 1741-7007. PMC 7820539. PMID 33482803. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7820539

  139. Elliott, Julian; Lawrence, Rebecca; Minx, Jan C.; Oladapo, Olufemi T.; Ravaud, Philippe; Tendal Jeppesen, Britta; Thomas, James; Turner, Tari; Vandvik, Per Olav; Grimshaw, Jeremy M. (December 2021). "Decision makers need constantly updated evidence synthesis". Nature. 600 (7889): 383–385. Bibcode:2021Natur.600..383E. doi:10.1038/d41586-021-03690-1. PMID 34912079. S2CID 245220047. https://doi.org/10.1038%2Fd41586-021-03690-1

  140. Gurevitch, Jessica; Koricheva, Julia; Nakagawa, Shinichi; Stewart, Gavin (March 2018). "Meta-analysis and the science of research synthesis". Nature. 555 (7695): 175–182. Bibcode:2018Natur.555..175G. doi:10.1038/nature25753. hdl:1959.4/unsworks_71830. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 29517004. S2CID 3761687. /wiki/Bibcode_(identifier)

  141. Fortunato, Santo; Bergstrom, Carl T.; Börner, Katy; Evans, James A.; Helbing, Dirk; Milojević, Staša; Petersen, Alexander M.; Radicchi, Filippo; Sinatra, Roberta; Uzzi, Brian; Vespignani, Alessandro; Waltman, Ludo; Wang, Dashun; Barabási, Albert-László (2 March 2018). "Science of science". Science. 359 (6379): eaao0185. doi:10.1126/science.aao0185. PMC 5949209. PMID 29496846. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323502497

  142. Fortunato, Santo; Bergstrom, Carl T.; Börner, Katy; Evans, James A.; Helbing, Dirk; Milojević, Staša; Petersen, Alexander M.; Radicchi, Filippo; Sinatra, Roberta; Uzzi, Brian; Vespignani, Alessandro; Waltman, Ludo; Wang, Dashun; Barabási, Albert-László (2 March 2018). "Science of science". Science. 359 (6379): eaao0185. doi:10.1126/science.aao0185. PMC 5949209. PMID 29496846. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323502497

  143. Krauss, A (2024). Science of Science: Understanding the Foundations and Limits of Science from an Interdisciplinary Perspective. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/9780198937401.001.0001. ISBN 978-0-19-893740-1. 978-0-19-893740-1

  144. Snyder, Alison (14 October 2021). "New ideas are struggling to emerge from the sea of science". Axios. Retrieved 15 November 2021. https://www.axios.com/science-new-ideas-dbe29601-010c-411a-b79d-bbd1388ec5a0.html

  145. Chu, Johan S. G.; Evans, James A. (12 October 2021). "Slowed canonical progress in large fields of science". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 118 (41): e2021636118. Bibcode:2021PNAS..11821636C. doi:10.1073/pnas.2021636118. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 8522281. PMID 34607941. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8522281

  146. Fortunato, Santo; Bergstrom, Carl T.; Börner, Katy; Evans, James A.; Helbing, Dirk; Milojević, Staša; Petersen, Alexander M.; Radicchi, Filippo; Sinatra, Roberta; Uzzi, Brian; Vespignani, Alessandro; Waltman, Ludo; Wang, Dashun; Barabási, Albert-László (2 March 2018). "Science of science". Science. 359 (6379): eaao0185. doi:10.1126/science.aao0185. PMC 5949209. PMID 29496846. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323502497

  147. "Sharing of tacit knowledge is most important aspect of mentorship, study finds". phys.org. Retrieved 4 July 2020. https://phys.org/news/2020-06-tacit-knowledge-important-aspect-mentorship.html

  148. Ma, Yifang; Mukherjee, Satyam; Uzzi, Brian (23 June 2020). "Mentorship and protégé success in STEM fields". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 117 (25): 14077–14083. Bibcode:2020PNAS..11714077M. doi:10.1073/pnas.1915516117. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 7322065. PMID 32522881. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC7322065

  149. "Science of Science authors hope to spark conversations about the scientific enterprise". phys.org. Retrieved 28 January 2023. https://phys.org/news/2018-03-science-authors-conversations-scientific-enterprise.html

  150. Fortunato, Santo; Bergstrom, Carl T.; Börner, Katy; Evans, James A.; Helbing, Dirk; Milojević, Staša; Petersen, Alexander M.; Radicchi, Filippo; Sinatra, Roberta; Uzzi, Brian; Vespignani, Alessandro; Waltman, Ludo; Wang, Dashun; Barabási, Albert-László (2 March 2018). "Science of science". Science. 359 (6379): eaao0185. doi:10.1126/science.aao0185. PMC 5949209. PMID 29496846. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323502497

  151. van Dijk, Peter J.; Jessop, Adrienne P.; Ellis, T. H. Noel (July 2022). "How did Mendel arrive at his discoveries?". Nature Genetics. 54 (7): 926–933. doi:10.1038/s41588-022-01109-9. ISSN 1546-1718. PMID 35817970. S2CID 250454204. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  152. Fortunato, Santo; Bergstrom, Carl T.; Börner, Katy; Evans, James A.; Helbing, Dirk; Milojević, Staša; Petersen, Alexander M.; Radicchi, Filippo; Sinatra, Roberta; Uzzi, Brian; Vespignani, Alessandro; Waltman, Ludo; Wang, Dashun; Barabási, Albert-László (2 March 2018). "Science of science". Science. 359 (6379): eaao0185. doi:10.1126/science.aao0185. PMC 5949209. PMID 29496846. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323502497

  153. Root-Bernstein, Robert S.; Bernstein, Maurine; Garnier, Helen (1 April 1995). "Correlations Between Avocations, Scientific Style, Work Habits, and Professional Impact of Scientists". Creativity Research Journal. 8 (2): 115–137. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj0802_2. ISSN 1040-0419. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  154. Ince, Sharon; Hoadley, Christopher; Kirschner, Paul A. (1 January 2022). "A qualitative study of social sciences faculty research workflows". Journal of Documentation. 78 (6): 1321–1337. doi:10.1108/JD-08-2021-0168. ISSN 0022-0418. S2CID 247078086. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  155. Nassi-Calò, Lilian (3 April 2014). "Researchers reading habits for scientific literature | SciELO in Perspective". Retrieved 25 February 2023. https://blog.scielo.org/en/2014/04/03/researchers-reading-habits-for-scientific-literature/

  156. Van Noorden, Richard (3 February 2014). "Scientists may be reaching a peak in reading habits". Nature. doi:10.1038/nature.2014.14658. Retrieved 25 February 2023. https://www.nature.com/articles/nature.2014.14658

  157. Arshad, Alia; Ameen, Kanwal (1 January 2021). "Comparative analysis of academic scientists, social scientists and humanists' scholarly information seeking habits". The Journal of Academic Librarianship. 47 (1): 102297. doi:10.1016/j.acalib.2020.102297. ISSN 0099-1333. S2CID 229433047. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  158. "Why it pays to join a big research group if you want to be more scientifically productive". Physics World. 24 November 2022. Retrieved 13 December 2022. https://physicsworld.com/a/why-it-pays-to-join-a-big-research-group-if-you-want-to-be-more-scientifically-productive/

  159. Zhang, Sam; Wapman, K. Hunter; Larremore, Daniel B.; Clauset, Aaron (16 November 2022). "Labor advantages drive the greater productivity of faculty at elite universities". Science Advances. 8 (46): eabq7056. arXiv:2204.05989. Bibcode:2022SciA....8.7056Z. doi:10.1126/sciadv.abq7056. ISSN 2375-2548. PMC 9674273. PMID 36399560. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9674273

  160. Dunn, Katharine. "LibGuides: Promotion & Tenure and Open Scholarship: How is "impact" measured & valued?". libguides.mit.edu. Retrieved 2024-03-14. https://libguides.mit.edu/c.php?g=1162307&p=8489698

  161. Gurevitch, Jessica; Koricheva, Julia; Nakagawa, Shinichi; Stewart, Gavin (March 2018). "Meta-analysis and the science of research synthesis". Nature. 555 (7695): 175–182. Bibcode:2018Natur.555..175G. doi:10.1038/nature25753. hdl:1959.4/unsworks_71830. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 29517004. S2CID 3761687. /wiki/Bibcode_(identifier)

  162. "Academic Incentives and Research Impact: Developing Reward and Recognition Systems to Better People's Lives". DORA. Retrieved 28 January 2023. https://sfdora.org/resource/academic-incentives-and-research-impact-developing-reward-and-recognition-systems-to-better-peoples-lives/

  163. Collison, Patrick; Cowen, Tyler (30 July 2019). "We Need a New Science of Progress". The Atlantic. Retrieved 25 January 2023. https://www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2019/07/we-need-new-science-progress/594946/

  164. Lovely, Garrison (16 June 2022). "Do we need a better understanding of 'progress'?". BBC. Retrieved 27 January 2023. https://www.bbc.com/future/article/20220615-do-we-need-a-better-understanding-of-progress

  165. Khalil, Mohammed M. (2016). "Improving Science for a Better Future". How Should Humanity Steer the Future?. The Frontiers Collection. Springer International Publishing. pp. 113–126. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-20717-9_11. ISBN 978-3-319-20716-2. 978-3-319-20716-2

  166. Niehaus, Paul; Williams, Heidi. "Developing the science of science". Works in Progress. Retrieved 25 January 2023. https://www.worksinprogress.co/issue/developing-the-science-of-science/

  167. "How to escape scientific stagnation". The Economist. Retrieved 25 January 2023. https://www.economist.com/finance-and-economics/2022/10/26/how-to-escape-scientific-stagnation

  168. Ioannidis, John P. A.; Fanelli, Daniele; Dunne, Debbie Drake; Goodman, Steven N. (2 October 2015). "Meta-research: Evaluation and Improvement of Research Methods and Practices". PLOS Biology. 13 (10): e1002264. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1002264. ISSN 1544-9173. PMC 4592065. PMID 26431313. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4592065

  169. "Registered Replication Reports". Association for Psychological Science. Retrieved 2015-11-13. http://www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/replication

  170. Chambers, Chris (2014-05-20). "Psychology's 'registration revolution'". the Guardian. Retrieved 2015-11-13. https://www.theguardian.com/science/head-quarters/2014/may/20/psychology-registration-revolution

  171. Simera, I; Moher, D; Hirst, A; Hoey, J; Schulz, KF; Altman, DG (2010). "Transparent and accurate reporting increases reliability, utility, and impact of your research: reporting guidelines and the EQUATOR Network". BMC Medicine. 8: 24. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-8-24. PMC 2874506. PMID 20420659. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2874506

  172. Simera, I.; Moher, D.; Hoey, J.; Schulz, K. F.; Altman, D. G. (2010). "A catalogue of reporting guidelines for health research". European Journal of Clinical Investigation. 40 (1): 35–53. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2362.2009.02234.x. PMID 20055895. https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1365-2362.2009.02234.x

  173. Simera, I; Altman, DG (October 2009). "Writing a research article that is "fit for purpose": EQUATOR Network and reporting guidelines". Evidence-Based Medicine. 14 (5): 132–134. doi:10.1136/ebm.14.5.132. PMID 19794009. S2CID 36739841. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  174. Ep. 49: Joel Chan on metascience, creativity, and tools for thought. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=KT-I_6TERKk

  175. Fortunato, Santo; Bergstrom, Carl T.; Börner, Katy; Evans, James A.; Helbing, Dirk; Milojević, Staša; Petersen, Alexander M.; Radicchi, Filippo; Sinatra, Roberta; Uzzi, Brian; Vespignani, Alessandro; Waltman, Ludo; Wang, Dashun; Barabási, Albert-László (2 March 2018). "Science of science". Science. 359 (6379): eaao0185. doi:10.1126/science.aao0185. PMC 5949209. PMID 29496846. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323502497

  176. "Risk of Bias Tool | Cochrane Bias". methods.cochrane.org. Retrieved 25 January 2023. https://methods.cochrane.org/bias/risk-bias-tool

  177. Prasad, Vinay; Ioannidis, John P. A. (November 2022). "Constructive and obsessive criticism in science". European Journal of Clinical Investigation. 52 (11): e13839. doi:10.1111/eci.13839. ISSN 0014-2972. PMC 9787955. PMID 35869811. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9787955

  178. Chan, Joel; Lutters, Wayne; Schneider, Jodi; Kirsanow, Karola; Bessa, Silvia; Saunders, Jonny L. (8 November 2022). "Growing New Scholarly Communication Infrastructures for Sharing, Reusing, and Synthesizing Knowledge". Companion Computer Supported Cooperative Work and Social Computing. Association for Computing Machinery. pp. 278–281. doi:10.1145/3500868.3559398. ISBN 9781450391900. S2CID 253385733. 9781450391900

  179. Segado-Boj, Francisco; Martín-Quevedo, Juan; Prieto-Gutiérrez, Juan-José (12 December 2022). "Jumping over the paywall: Strategies and motivations for scholarly piracy and other alternatives" (PDF). Information Development. 40 (3): 442–460. doi:10.1177/02666669221144429. ISSN 0266-6669. S2CID 254564205. https://eprints.ucm.es/id/eprint/75874/1/Preprint_Segado-Boj_ID_2022.pdf

  180. Gosztyla, Maya (7 July 2022). "How to find, read and organize papers". Nature. doi:10.1038/d41586-022-01878-7. PMID 35804061. S2CID 250388551. Retrieved 28 January 2023. https://www.nature.com/articles/d41586-022-01878-7

  181. Fastrez, Pierre; Jacques, Jerry (2015). "Managing References by Filing and Tagging: An Exploratory Study of Personal Information Management by Social Scientists". Human Interface and the Management of Information. Information and Knowledge Design. Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Vol. 9172. Springer International Publishing. pp. 291–300. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-20612-7_28. ISBN 978-3-319-20611-0. 978-3-319-20611-0

  182. Chaudhry, Abdus Sattar; Alajmi, Bibi M. (1 January 2022). "Personal information management practices: how scientists find and organize information". Global Knowledge, Memory and Communication. 73 (6/7): 757–774. doi:10.1108/GKMC-04-2022-0082. S2CID 253363619. https://www.emerald.com/insight/content/doi/10.1108/GKMC-04-2022-0082/full/html

  183. Chang, Joseph Chee; Kim, Yongsung; Miller, Victor; Liu, Michael Xieyang; Myers, Brad A; Kittur, Aniket (12 October 2021). "Tabs.do: Task-Centric Browser Tab Management". The 34th Annual ACM Symposium on User Interface Software and Technology. Association for Computing Machinery. pp. 663–676. doi:10.1145/3472749.3474777. ISBN 9781450386357. S2CID 237102658. 9781450386357

  184. Fortunato, Santo; Bergstrom, Carl T.; Börner, Katy; Evans, James A.; Helbing, Dirk; Milojević, Staša; Petersen, Alexander M.; Radicchi, Filippo; Sinatra, Roberta; Uzzi, Brian; Vespignani, Alessandro; Waltman, Ludo; Wang, Dashun; Barabási, Albert-László (2 March 2018). "Science of science". Science. 359 (6379): eaao0185. doi:10.1126/science.aao0185. PMC 5949209. PMID 29496846. Retrieved 22 November 2021. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/323502497

  185. Rasberry, Lane; Tibbs, Sheri; Hoos, William; Westermann, Amy; Keefer, Jeffrey; Baskauf, Steven James; Anderson, Clifford; Walker, Philip; Kwok, Cherrie; Mietchen, Daniel (4 April 2022). "WikiProject Clinical Trials for Wikidata". medRxiv. doi:10.1101/2022.04.01.22273328. S2CID 247936371. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  186. Moral-Muñoz, José A.; Herrera-Viedma, Enrique; Santisteban-Espejo, Antonio; Cobo, Manuel J. (19 January 2020). "Software tools for conducting bibliometric analysis in science: An up-to-date review". El Profesional de la Información. 29 (1). doi:10.3145/epi.2020.ene.03. hdl:10481/62406. S2CID 210926828. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  187. "A new replication crisis: Research that is less likely to be true is cited more". phys.org. Retrieved 14 June 2021. https://phys.org/news/2021-05-replication-crisis-true-cited.html

  188. Serra-Garcia, Marta; Gneezy, Uri (2021-05-01). "Nonreplicable publications are cited more than replicable ones". Science Advances. 7 (21): eabd1705. Bibcode:2021SciA....7.1705S. doi:10.1126/sciadv.abd1705. ISSN 2375-2548. PMC 8139580. PMID 34020944. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8139580

  189. Parker, Lisa; Boughton, Stephanie; Lawrence, Rosa; Bero, Lisa (1 November 2022). "Experts identified warning signs of fraudulent research: a qualitative study to inform a screening tool". Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 151: 1–17. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2022.07.006. PMID 35850426. S2CID 250632662. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  190. Ioannidis, JPA (2016). "Why Most Clinical Research Is Not Useful". PLOS Med. 13 (6): e1002049. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1002049. PMC 4915619. PMID 27328301. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4915619

  191. Ioannidis JA (13 July 2005). "Contradicted and initially stronger effects in highly cited clinical research". JAMA. 294 (2): 218–228. doi:10.1001/jama.294.2.218. PMID 16014596. https://doi.org/10.1001%2Fjama.294.2.218

  192. Chalmers, Iain; Glasziou, Paul (2009). "Avoidable waste in the production and reporting of research evidence". The Lancet. 374 (9683): 86–89. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60329-9. ISSN 0140-6736. PMID 19525005. S2CID 11797088. http://timetravel.mementoweb.org/memento/2009/http://www.thelancet.com/journals/lancet

  193. June 24, Jeremy Hsu; ET, Jeremy Hsu (24 June 2010). "Dark Side of Medical Research: Widespread Bias and Omissions". Live Science. Retrieved 24 May 2019.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link) https://www.livescience.com/8365-dark-side-medical-research-widespread-bias-omissions.html

  194. "Confronting conflict of interest". Nature Medicine. 24 (11): 1629. November 2018. doi:10.1038/s41591-018-0256-7. ISSN 1546-170X. PMID 30401866. https://doi.org/10.1038%2Fs41591-018-0256-7

  195. Haque, Waqas; Minhajuddin, Abu; Gupta, Arjun; Agrawal, Deepak (2018). "Conflicts of interest of editors of medical journals". PLOS ONE. 13 (5): e0197141. Bibcode:2018PLoSO..1397141H. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0197141. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 5959187. PMID 29775468. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5959187

  196. Moncrieff, J (March 2002). "The antidepressant debate". The British Journal of Psychiatry. 180 (3): 193–194. doi:10.1192/bjp.180.3.193. ISSN 0007-1250. PMID 11872507. https://doi.org/10.1192%2Fbjp.180.3.193

  197. Bello, S; Moustgaard, H; Hróbjartsson, A (October 2014). "The risk of unblinding was infrequently and incompletely reported in 300 randomized clinical trial publications". Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 67 (10): 1059–1069. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2014.05.007. ISSN 1878-5921. PMID 24973822. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  198. Tuleu, Catherine; Legay, Helene; Orlu-Gul, Mine; Wan, Mandy (1 September 2013). "Blinding in pharmacological trials: the devil is in the details". Archives of Disease in Childhood. 98 (9): 656–659. doi:10.1136/archdischild-2013-304037. ISSN 0003-9888. PMC 3833301. PMID 23898156. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3833301

  199. Kirsch, I (2014). "Antidepressants and the Placebo Effect". Zeitschrift für Psychologie. 222 (3): 128–134. doi:10.1027/2151-2604/a000176. ISSN 2190-8370. PMC 4172306. PMID 25279271. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4172306

  200. Ioannidis, John PA (27 May 2008). "Effectiveness of antidepressants: an evidence myth constructed from a thousand randomized trials?". Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine. 3: 14. doi:10.1186/1747-5341-3-14. ISSN 1747-5341. PMC 2412901. PMID 18505564. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2412901

  201. Moher, David; Altman, Douglas G.; Schulz, Kenneth F. (24 March 2010). "CONSORT 2010 Statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials". BMJ. 340: c332. doi:10.1136/bmj.c332. ISSN 0959-8138. PMC 2844940. PMID 20332509. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2844940

  202. Clarke, Michael; Chalmers, Iain (1998). "Discussion Sections in Reports of Controlled Trials Published in General Medical Journals". JAMA. 280 (3): 280–282. doi:10.1001/jama.280.3.280. PMID 9676682. https://doi.org/10.1001%2Fjama.280.3.280

  203. Lau, Joseph; Antman, Elliott M; Jimenez-Silva, Jeanette; Kupelnick, Bruce; Mosteller, Frederick; Chalmers, Thomas C (1992). "Cumulative Meta-Analysis of Therapeutic Trials for Myocardial Infarction". New England Journal of Medicine. 327 (4): 248–254. doi:10.1056/NEJM199207233270406. PMID 1614465. https://doi.org/10.1056%2FNEJM199207233270406

  204. Fergusson, Dean; Glass, Kathleen Cranley; Hutton, Brian; Shapiro, Stan (2016). "Randomized controlled trials of aprotinin in cardiac surgery: Could clinical equipoise have stopped the bleeding?". Clinical Trials. 2 (3): 218–229, discussion 229–232. doi:10.1191/1740774505cn085oa. PMID 16279145. S2CID 31375469. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  205. Clarke, Mike; Brice, Anne; Chalmers, Iain (2014). "Accumulating Research: A Systematic Account of How Cumulative Meta-Analyses Would Have Provided Knowledge, Improved Health, Reduced Harm and Saved Resources". PLOS ONE. 9 (7): e102670. Bibcode:2014PLoSO...9j2670C. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0102670. PMC 4113310. PMID 25068257. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4113310

  206. Lau, Joseph; Antman, Elliott M; Jimenez-Silva, Jeanette; Kupelnick, Bruce; Mosteller, Frederick; Chalmers, Thomas C (1992). "Cumulative Meta-Analysis of Therapeutic Trials for Myocardial Infarction". New England Journal of Medicine. 327 (4): 248–254. doi:10.1056/NEJM199207233270406. PMID 1614465. https://doi.org/10.1056%2FNEJM199207233270406

  207. Robinson, Karen A; Goodman, Steven N (2011). "A Systematic Examination of the Citation of Prior Research in Reports of Randomized, Controlled Trials". Annals of Internal Medicine. 154 (1): 50–55. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-154-1-201101040-00007. PMID 21200038. S2CID 207536137. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  208. Robinson, Karen A; Goodman, Steven N (2011). "A Systematic Examination of the Citation of Prior Research in Reports of Randomized, Controlled Trials". Annals of Internal Medicine. 154 (1): 50–55. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-154-1-201101040-00007. PMID 21200038. S2CID 207536137. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  209. Epstein, David. "When Evidence Says No, but Doctors Say Yes - The Atlantic". Pocket. Retrieved 10 April 2020. https://getpocket.com/explore/item/when-evidence-says-no-but-doctors-say-yes

  210. Tatsioni, A; Bonitsis, NG; Ioannidis, JP (5 December 2007). "Persistence of contradicted claims in the literature". JAMA. 298 (21): 2517–2526. doi:10.1001/jama.298.21.2517. ISSN 1538-3598. PMID 18056905. https://doi.org/10.1001%2Fjama.298.21.2517

  211. Franco, Annie; Malhotra, Neil; Simonovits, Gabor (1 January 2016). "Underreporting in Psychology Experiments: Evidence From a Study Registry". Social Psychological and Personality Science. 7 (1): 8–12. doi:10.1177/1948550615598377. ISSN 1948-5506. S2CID 143182733. /wiki/Neil_Malhotra

  212. Munafò, Marcus (29 March 2017). "Metascience: Reproducibility blues". Nature. 543 (7647): 619–620. Bibcode:2017Natur.543..619M. doi:10.1038/543619a. ISSN 1476-4687. https://doi.org/10.1038%2F543619a

  213. Stokstad, Erik (20 September 2018). "This research group seeks to expose weaknesses in science – and they'll step on some toes if they have to". Science. doi:10.1126/science.aav4784. S2CID 158525979. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  214. Open Science Collaboration (2015). "Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science" (PDF). Science. 349 (6251): aac4716. doi:10.1126/science.aac4716. hdl:10722/230596. PMID 26315443. S2CID 218065162. http://eprints.keele.ac.uk/877/1/Open%20Science%20%28Science%20Pre-Print%29.pdf

  215. Allen, Christopher P G.; Mehler, David Marc Anton. "Open Science challenges, benefits and tips in early career and beyond". doi:10.31234/osf.io/3czyt. S2CID 240061030. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help) http://psyarxiv.com/3czyt/

  216. Simmons, Joseph P.; Nelson, Leif D.; Simonsohn, Uri (2011). "False-Positive Psychology". Psychological Science. 22 (11): 1359–1366. doi:10.1177/0956797611417632. PMID 22006061. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0956797611417632

  217. Stroebe, Wolfgang; Strack, Fritz (2014). "The Alleged Crisis and the Illusion of Exact Replication" (PDF). Perspectives on Psychological Science. 9 (1): 59–71. doi:10.1177/1745691613514450. PMID 26173241. S2CID 31938129. https://pure.rug.nl/ws/files/12588700/postprint_Stroebe_Strack_2014.pdf

  218. Aschwanden, Christie (6 December 2018). "Psychology's Replication Crisis Has Made The Field Better". FiveThirtyEight. Retrieved 19 December 2018. /wiki/Christie_Aschwanden

  219. Allen, Christopher P G.; Mehler, David Marc Anton. "Open Science challenges, benefits and tips in early career and beyond". doi:10.31234/osf.io/3czyt. S2CID 240061030. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help) http://psyarxiv.com/3czyt/

  220. Cohen, Jacob (1994). "The earth is round (p < .05)". American Psychologist. 49 (12): 997–1003. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.49.12.997. S2CID 380942. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  221. MacCoun, Robert; Perlmutter, Saul (8 October 2015). "Blind analysis: Hide results to seek the truth". Nature. 526 (7572): 187–189. Bibcode:2015Natur.526..187M. doi:10.1038/526187a. PMID 26450040. https://doi.org/10.1038%2F526187a

  222. Demir, Nurullah; Große-Kampmann, Matteo; Urban, Tobias; Wressnegger, Christian; Holz, Thorsten; Pohlmann, Norbert (2022). "Reproducibility and Replicability of Web Measurement Studies". Proceedings of the ACM Web Conference 2022. WWW '22. New York: Association for Computing Machinery. pp. 533–544. doi:10.1145/3485447.3512214. /wiki/Doi_(identifier)

  223. Berlin, Meta-Research Innovation Center. "Meta-Research Innovation Center Berlin". Meta-Research Innovation Center Berlin. Retrieved 2021-12-06. https://www.metricberlin.bihealth.org/

  224. "Home | Meta-research Innovation Center at Stanford". metrics.stanford.edu. Retrieved 2021-12-06. https://metrics.stanford.edu/

  225. "Meta-research and Evidence Synthesis Unit". The George Institute for Global Health. Retrieved 2021-12-19. https://www.georgeinstitute.org.in/units/meta-research-and-evidence-synthesis-unit

  226. "AIMOS 2022". AIMOS 2022. Retrieved 20 March 2023. https://www.eventcreate.com/e/aimos2022

  227. "Metascience 2023". Metascience 2023 Conference. Retrieved 20 March 2023. https://metascience.info/